全文获取类型
收费全文 | 28139篇 |
免费 | 1118篇 |
国内免费 | 9篇 |
专业分类
29266篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 283篇 |
2019年 | 313篇 |
2018年 | 451篇 |
2017年 | 495篇 |
2016年 | 508篇 |
2015年 | 376篇 |
2014年 | 454篇 |
2013年 | 2224篇 |
2012年 | 800篇 |
2011年 | 848篇 |
2010年 | 546篇 |
2009年 | 522篇 |
2008年 | 773篇 |
2007年 | 697篇 |
2006年 | 688篇 |
2005年 | 582篇 |
2004年 | 563篇 |
2003年 | 586篇 |
2002年 | 568篇 |
2001年 | 680篇 |
2000年 | 692篇 |
1999年 | 557篇 |
1998年 | 299篇 |
1997年 | 313篇 |
1996年 | 306篇 |
1995年 | 292篇 |
1992年 | 509篇 |
1991年 | 470篇 |
1990年 | 461篇 |
1989年 | 429篇 |
1988年 | 455篇 |
1987年 | 417篇 |
1986年 | 461篇 |
1985年 | 441篇 |
1984年 | 400篇 |
1983年 | 348篇 |
1979年 | 413篇 |
1978年 | 336篇 |
1977年 | 291篇 |
1976年 | 306篇 |
1975年 | 345篇 |
1974年 | 433篇 |
1973年 | 470篇 |
1972年 | 348篇 |
1971年 | 369篇 |
1970年 | 335篇 |
1969年 | 363篇 |
1968年 | 441篇 |
1967年 | 405篇 |
1966年 | 421篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
11.
12.
Christopher D. Adams Anthony Dickinson 《The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology》1981,33(2):109-121
In two experiments, hungry rats were given instrumental lever-press training for an appetitive reinforcer and, in addition, were exposed to another type of food which was not contingent on lever pressing. In the first experiment, exposure to each type of food was on separate days, whereas in the second experiment rats were exposed to each type of food in strict alternation within each session. Subsequently, a food aversion was conditioned to the reinforcer for the experimental group and to the non-contingent food for the control group. In both experiments, animals with an aversion to the reinforcer responded less in an extinction test than animals with an aversion to the non-contingent food. Subsequent reacquisition tests confirmed that the aversion to the non-contingent food in the control group was of comparable strength with that to the reinforcer in the experimental group. The results were discussed in terms of whether the reinforcer is encoded in the associative structure set up by exposure to an instrumental contingency. 相似文献
13.
14.
15.
Abby C King Bess Marcus David Ahn Andrea L Dunn W Jack Rejeski James F Sallis Mace Coday 《Health psychology》2006,25(3):336-347
The authors used recursive partitioning methods to identify combinations of baseline characteristics that predict 2-year physical activity success in each of 3 physical activity interventions delivered in the multisite Activity Counseling Trial. The sample consisted of 874 initially sedentary primary care patients, ages 35-75 years, who were at risk for cardiovascular disease. Predictors of 2-year success were specific to each intervention and represented a range of domains, including physiological, demographic, psychosocial, health-related, and environmental variables. The results indicate how specific patient subgroups (e.g., obese, unfit individuals; high-income individuals in stable health) may respond differently to varying levels and amounts of professional assistance and support. The methods used provide a practical first step toward identifying clinically meaningful patient subgroups for further systematic investigation. 相似文献
16.
Marcellus M Merritt Gary G Bennett Redford B Williams Christopher L Edwards John J Sollers 《Health psychology》2006,25(3):364-369
This study evaluated cardiovascular responses (CVR) to an active speech task with blatantly discriminatory (BRC) versus neutral (NRC) stimuli and an anger recall task in a sample of Black men (N = 73; age 18 to 47). Diastolic blood pressure scores were higher for NRC versus BRC stimuli during anger recall (p = .05). Moreover, persons in the NRC group who perceived high levels of racism (vs. no racism or BRC group) during active speech showed larger increases in blood pressure across postspeech rest, anger recall, and subsequent rest (p = .03). The notable elevation in CVR in response to an ambiguous event extends current models of racism suggesting that subtle racism is a psychosocial stressor that erodes health through chronically elevated CVR. 相似文献
17.
18.
19.
20.
S Sipe R W Engle 《Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition》1986,12(3):402-412
Four experiments were conducted to determine whether echoic memory plays a role in differences between good and poor readers. The first two experiments used a suffix procedure in which the subject is read a list of digits with either a tone control or the word go appended to the list. For lists that exceeded the length of the subjects memory span by one digit (i.e., that avoided ceiling effects), the poor readers showed a larger decrement in the suffix condition than did the good readers. The third experiment was directed at the question of whether the duration of echoic memory is different for good and poor readers. Children shadowed words presented to one ear at a rate determined to give 75-85% shadowing accuracy. The items presented to the nonattended ear were words and an occasional digit. At various intervals after the presentation of the digit, a light signaled that the subject was to cease shadowing and attempt to recall any digit that had occurred in the nonattended ear recently. Whereas good and poor readers recalled the digit equally if tested immediately after presentation, the poor readers showed a faster decline in recall of the digit as retention interval increased. A fourth experiment was conducted to determine whether the differences in echoic memory were specific to speech stimuli or occurred at a more basic level of aural persistence. Bursts of white noise were separated by 9-400 ms of silence and the subject was to say whether there were one or two sounds presented. There were no differences in detectability functions for good and poor readers. 相似文献