全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3773篇 |
免费 | 64篇 |
专业分类
3837篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 27篇 |
2023年 | 28篇 |
2022年 | 20篇 |
2021年 | 53篇 |
2020年 | 95篇 |
2019年 | 100篇 |
2018年 | 134篇 |
2017年 | 146篇 |
2016年 | 148篇 |
2015年 | 120篇 |
2014年 | 134篇 |
2013年 | 486篇 |
2012年 | 228篇 |
2011年 | 223篇 |
2010年 | 122篇 |
2009年 | 135篇 |
2008年 | 175篇 |
2007年 | 155篇 |
2006年 | 165篇 |
2005年 | 130篇 |
2004年 | 114篇 |
2003年 | 126篇 |
2002年 | 93篇 |
2001年 | 40篇 |
2000年 | 48篇 |
1999年 | 49篇 |
1998年 | 47篇 |
1997年 | 37篇 |
1996年 | 35篇 |
1995年 | 34篇 |
1994年 | 32篇 |
1993年 | 42篇 |
1992年 | 20篇 |
1991年 | 23篇 |
1990年 | 18篇 |
1989年 | 11篇 |
1988年 | 15篇 |
1987年 | 11篇 |
1986年 | 15篇 |
1985年 | 16篇 |
1984年 | 26篇 |
1983年 | 13篇 |
1982年 | 18篇 |
1981年 | 16篇 |
1980年 | 14篇 |
1979年 | 12篇 |
1978年 | 12篇 |
1977年 | 11篇 |
1976年 | 14篇 |
1972年 | 10篇 |
排序方式: 共有3837条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
191.
Cheever KH Jubilan B Dailey T Ehrhardt K Blumenstein R Morin CJ Lewis C 《Journal of religion and health》2005,44(1):67-80
This study aimed to identify the religious practices and beliefs of surgeons and the relationship between surgeons locus of control and religiosity. Thirty-five surgeons completed a survey that included items from the Duke University Religion Index, the Salesian Center Intrinsic Religiosity Scale for Clinicians, and Rotters Locus of Control Scale. Over 68% of sampled surgeons affirmed that their religious beliefs play a part in their practice, 47% attend religious services at least weekly, and 44% pray daily. There was no correlation between locus of control and religiosity. These results challenge the myth of the egocentric, agnostic surgeon. 相似文献
192.
193.
Eric L. G. Legge Christopher R. Madan Marcia L. Spetch Elliot A. Ludvig 《Animal cognition》2016,19(3):581-591
Encoding multiple cues can improve the accuracy and reliability of navigation and goal localization. Problems may arise, however, if one cue is displaced and provides information which conflicts with other cues. Here we investigated how pigeons cope with cue conflict by training them to locate a goal relative to two landmarks and then varying the amount of conflict between the landmarks. When the amount of conflict was small, pigeons tended to integrate both cues in their search patterns. When the amount of conflict was large, however, pigeons used information from both cues independently. This context-dependent strategy for resolving spatial cue conflict agrees with Bayes optimal calculations for using information from multiple sources. 相似文献
194.
Christopher Cowie 《Ethical Theory and Moral Practice》2016,19(3):711-723
Some subjectivist views of practical reasons entail that some people, in some cases, lack sufficient reasons to act as morality requires of them. This is often thought to form the basis of an objection to these subjectivist views: ‘the amoralism objection’. This objection has been developed at length by Julia Markovits in her recent book Moral Reason. But Markovits—alongside many other proponents of this objection—does not explicitly consider that her objection is premised on a claim that her opponents deny on first-order grounds, often as part of a socially and politically motivated revisionism about the assessment of agents and their actions. As such, the amoralism objection as she presents it misses its dialectical mark. This has interesting consequences for subjectivism—and the methodology behind it—more generally. 相似文献
195.
196.
Christopher J. G. Meacham 《Philosophical Studies》2008,138(2):245-269
This paper examines three accounts of the sleeping beauty case: an account proposed by Adam Elga, an account proposed by David
Lewis, and a third account defended in this paper. It provides two reasons for preferring the third account. First, this account
does a good job of capturing the temporal continuity of our beliefs, while the accounts favored by Elga and Lewis do not.
Second, Elga’s and Lewis’ treatments of the sleeping beauty case lead to highly counterintuitive consequences. The proposed
account also leads to counterintuitive consequences, but they’re not as bad as those of Elga’s account, and no worse than
those of Lewis’ account.
相似文献
Christopher J. G. MeachamEmail: |
197.
We examined the effects of sensorimotor experience in two visual word recognition tasks. Body-object interaction (BOI) ratings were collected for a large set of words. These ratings assess perceptions of the ease with which a human body can physically interact with a word's referent. A set of high BOI words (e.g., mask) and a set of low BOI words (e.g., ship) were created, matched on imageability and concreteness. Facilitatory BOI effects were observed in lexical decision and phonological lexical decision tasks: responses were faster for high BOI words than for low BOI words. We discuss how our findings may be accounted for by (a) semantic feedback within the visual word recognition system, and (b) an embodied view of cognition (e.g., Barsalou's perceptual symbol systems theory), which proposes that semantic knowledge is grounded in sensorimotor interactions with the environment. 相似文献
198.
199.
200.