全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3930篇 |
免费 | 71篇 |
专业分类
4001篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 29篇 |
2023年 | 28篇 |
2022年 | 20篇 |
2021年 | 54篇 |
2020年 | 94篇 |
2019年 | 101篇 |
2018年 | 143篇 |
2017年 | 154篇 |
2016年 | 156篇 |
2015年 | 127篇 |
2014年 | 147篇 |
2013年 | 499篇 |
2012年 | 233篇 |
2011年 | 232篇 |
2010年 | 127篇 |
2009年 | 140篇 |
2008年 | 178篇 |
2007年 | 163篇 |
2006年 | 169篇 |
2005年 | 134篇 |
2004年 | 116篇 |
2003年 | 131篇 |
2002年 | 95篇 |
2001年 | 42篇 |
2000年 | 47篇 |
1999年 | 52篇 |
1998年 | 48篇 |
1997年 | 41篇 |
1996年 | 35篇 |
1995年 | 33篇 |
1994年 | 34篇 |
1993年 | 45篇 |
1992年 | 23篇 |
1991年 | 27篇 |
1990年 | 22篇 |
1989年 | 10篇 |
1988年 | 19篇 |
1987年 | 17篇 |
1986年 | 20篇 |
1985年 | 17篇 |
1984年 | 25篇 |
1983年 | 14篇 |
1982年 | 18篇 |
1981年 | 18篇 |
1980年 | 17篇 |
1979年 | 18篇 |
1978年 | 10篇 |
1977年 | 11篇 |
1976年 | 14篇 |
1972年 | 12篇 |
排序方式: 共有4001条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
241.
Blake Roeber 《No?s (Detroit, Mich.)》2020,54(4):839-866
Is knowledge consistent with literally any credence in the relevant proposition, including credence 0? Of course not. But is credence 0 the only credence in p that entails that you don't know that p? Knowledge entails belief (most epistemologists think), and it's impossible to believe that p while having credence 0 in p. Is it true that, for every value of ‘x,’ if it's impossible to know that p while having credence x in p, this is simply because it's impossible to believe that p while having credence x in p? If so, is it possible to believe that p while having (say) credence 0.4 in p? These questions aren't standard epistemological fare—at least in part because many epistemologists think their answers are obvious—but they have unanticipated consequences for epistemology. Let ‘improbabilism’ name the thesis that it's possible to know that p while having a credence in p below 0.5. Improbabilism will strike many epistemologists as absurd, but careful reflection on these questions reveals that, if improbabilism is false, then all of the most plausible theories of knowledge are also false. Or so I shall argue in this paper. Since improbabilism is widely rejected by epistemologists (at least implicitly), this paper reveals a tension between all of the most plausible theories of knowledge and a widespread assumption in epistemology. 相似文献
242.
Blake Roeber 《Philosophy and phenomenological research》2020,101(2):415-431
According to what I will call ‘the disanalogy thesis,’ beliefs differ from actions in at least the following important way: while cognitively healthy people often exhibit direct control over their actions, there is no possible scenario where a cognitively healthy person exhibits direct control over her beliefs. Recent arguments against the disanalogy thesis maintain that, if you find yourself in what I will call a ‘permissive situation’ with respect to p, then you can have direct control over whether you believe p, and you can do so without manifesting any cognitive defect. These arguments focus primarily on the idea that we can have direct doxastic control in permissive situations, but they provide insufficient reason for thinking that permissive situations are actually possible, since they pay inadequate attention to the following worries: permissive situations seem inconsistent with the uniqueness thesis, permissive situations seem inconsistent with natural thoughts about epistemic akrasia, and vagueness threatens even if we push these worries aside. In this paper I argue that, on the understanding of permissive situations that is most useful for evaluating the disanalogy thesis, permissive situations clearly are possible. 相似文献
243.
Spatial judgments are affected by both fine-grained and categorical knowledge. We investigated whether, and how, the two forms
of knowledge are learned in real-world, navigable space, as well as the time course of learning each type of knowledge. Participants
were Northwestern University undergraduates who estimated the locations of buildings and other landmarks on campus. The Northwestern
campus is roughly divided into three regions whose borders are not easy to discern, either from a map or by navigation. Nevertheless,
students often refer to these regions linguistically and use them when making housing decisions, choosing classes, and so
forth. We found that knowledge of both the fine-grained configuration of locations and the regional distinctions increased
with time. However, regional influences on judgments occurred later in students’ time on campus. Consequently, computed distances
across the nonexistent border between north and south campus locations became more biased with time. The results have implications
for understanding how spatial representations develop in navigable environments. 相似文献
244.
Christopher M. Spray Victoria E. Warburton Juliette Stebbings 《Psychology of sport and exercise》2013,14(5):662-669
ObjectivesThis study aimed to examine the effects of change in perceived teacher achievement goal emphasis in physical education (PE) on physical self-perceptions and self-esteem across the transition to secondary school.Design & methodsA longitudinal design was adopted with three time points, one at the end of primary school and two during the first year of secondary school. Participants (N = 491) were cross-classified by primary (N = 42) and secondary (N = 46) PE class in order to examine the association between perceived class-level teacher-emphasised goals and within-class student goals with self-beliefs.ResultsPersonal approach goals and class perceptions of teacher mastery approach goal promotion were all positively associated with ratings of co-ordination, sport competence, flexibility, and endurance in primary school. More favourable perceptions of coordination, sport competence, strength, flexibility, and endurance during the first year of secondary school were predicted by an increase in performance approach goal emphasis, whereas ratings of sport competence and flexibility were negatively associated with an increase in mastery approach goal emphasis.ConclusionsAlthough not entirely consonant with theoretical predictions, current findings suggest that teacher-emphasised performance approach goals in PE can promote development of several physical self-perceptions in the initial year of secondary school. 相似文献
245.
Christopher Toner 《The Journal of Ethics》2010,14(2):81-102
A survey of just war theory literature reveals the existence of quite different lists of principles. This apparent arbitrariness raises a number of questions: What is the relation between ad bellum and in bello principles? Why are there so many of the former and so few of the latter? What order is there among the various principles? To answer these questions, I first draw on some recent work by Jeff McMahan to show that ad bellum and in bello principles are not, as often portrayed, independent—the justice of conduct in war largely presupposes the justice of the recourse to war. Undermining this independence claim is one important step toward revealing the unified logical structure of just war theory. I then argue that we can see the dependence of the jus in bello upon the jus ad bellum, not just in the content of certain principles, but also in the structure of the two sets of principles: I construct a one-to-one mapping between ad bellum and in bello principles. In doing so, I argue also that the shared structure successfully finds place for the questions central to the evaluation of the morality of war: what is a sufficient provocation to use force, what objectives may be sought by force, why or for what ends, who has authority to decide to use force, and when or in what circumstances? Despite variations in expression, the theory allows for a coherent and comprehensive evaluation of morality in warfare. 相似文献
246.
Church BA Krauss MS Lopata C Toomey JA Thomeer ML Coutinho MV Volker MA Mercado E 《Psychonomic bulletin & review》2010,17(6):862-868
Children with autism spectrum disorder process many perceptual and social events differently from typically developing children,
suggesting that they may also form and recognize categories differently. We used a dot pattern categorization task and prototype
comparison modeling to compare categorical processing in children with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder and matched
typical controls. We were interested in whether there were differences in how children with autism use average similarity
information about a category to make decisions. During testing, the group with autism spectrum disorder endorsed prototypes
less and was seemingly less sensitive to differences between to-be-categorized items and the prototype. The findings suggest
that individuals with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder are less likely to use overall average similarity when forming
categories or making categorical decisions. Such differences in category formation and use may negatively impact processing
of socially relevant information, such as facial expressions. A supplemental appendix for this article may be downloaded from
http://pbr.psychonomic-journals.org/content/supplemental. 相似文献
247.
Christopher Grau 《The Southern journal of philosophy》2010,48(3):246-271
In this essay, I argue that a proper understanding of the historicity of love requires an appreciation of the irreplaceability of the beloved. I do this through a consideration of ideas that were first put forward by Robert Kraut in “Love De Re” (1986). I also evaluate Amelie Rorty's criticisms of Kraut's thesis in “The Historicity of Psychological Attitudes: Love is Not Love Which Alters Not When It Alteration Finds” (1986). I argue that Rorty fundamentally misunderstands Kraut's Kripkean analogy, and I go on to criticize her claim that concern over the proper object of love should be best understood as a concern over constancy. This leads me to an elaboration of the distinct senses in which love can be seen as historical. I end with a further defense of the irreplaceability of the beloved and a discussion of the relevance of recent debates over the importance of personal identity for an adequate account of the historical dimension of love. 相似文献
248.
Hilbert A Pike KM Wilfley DE Fairburn CG Dohm FA Striegel-Moore RH 《Behaviour research and therapy》2011,(3):202-211
Binge eating disorder (BED) presents with substantial psychiatric comorbidity. This latent structure analysis sought to delineate boundaries of BED given its comorbidity with affective and anxiety disorders. A population-based sample of 151 women with BED, 102 women with affective or anxiety disorders, and 259 women without psychiatric disorders was assessed with clinical interviews and self-report-questionnaires. Taxometric analyses were conducted using DSM-IV criteria of BED and of affective and anxiety disorders. The results showed a taxonic structure of BED and of affective and anxiety disorders. Both taxa co-occurred at an above-chance level, but also presented independently with twice-as-large probabilities. Within the BED taxon, diagnostic co-occurrence indicated greater general psychopathology, lower social adaptation, and greater premorbid exposure to parental mood and substance disorder, but not greater eating disorder psychopathology. Eating disorder psychopathology discriminated individuals in the BED taxon from individuals in the affective and anxiety disorders taxon. Diagnostic criteria of BED were more indicative of the BED taxon than were criteria of affective and anxiety disorders. The results show that at the latent level, BED was co-occurring with, yet distinct from, affective and anxiety disorders and was not characterized by an underlying affective or anxiety disorder. 相似文献
249.
Kimberly A.S. Howard Aaron H. Carlstrom Andrew D. Katz Aaronson Y. Chew G. Christopher Ray Lia Laine David Caulum 《Journal of Vocational Behavior》2011,(1):98-109
This study examined the influence of gender, socioeconomic status, and race/ethnicity on the career aspirations of over 22,000 8th and 10th grade youth. The top five occupations identified by youth as aspirations included artist, lawyer, musician, FBI agent, and actor/actress. Top occupations were also reported for each gender × socioeconomic status × race/ethnicity group. Aspirations were coded by social prestige level, minimum education requirements, and median salary. Results revealed significant main effects for socioeconomic status and race/ethnicity as well as significant interaction effects. Further, significant gender main effects and a significant gender × ethnicity interaction were found for occupational prestige and educational requirements. 相似文献
250.
Brascamp JW Blake R Kristjánsson Á 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》2011,37(6):1700-1707
With attention and eye-movements humans orient to targets of interest. This orienting occurs faster when the same target repeats: priming of pop-out (PoP). While reaction times (RTs) can be important, PoP's real function could be to steer where to orient, a possibility underexposed in many current paradigms, as these predesignate a target to which to orient. In a novel procedure we intermixed pop-out trials (one oddball target, two identical distractors) with choice trials (one item of each kind) where observers freely chose an item to attend to. Pop-out trials strongly drove subsequent choice: observers typically chose the preceding target. Conversely, choice trials affected subsequent pop-out RTs. Conventional PoP measures correlated positively with our choice measures among observers, suggesting common mechanisms. Our results support PoP accounts centered on altered target priority, and underscore PoP's importance for visual exploration. 相似文献