全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1908篇 |
免费 | 31篇 |
专业分类
1939篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 22篇 |
2023年 | 11篇 |
2022年 | 15篇 |
2021年 | 22篇 |
2020年 | 43篇 |
2019年 | 56篇 |
2018年 | 64篇 |
2017年 | 63篇 |
2016年 | 78篇 |
2015年 | 51篇 |
2014年 | 44篇 |
2013年 | 233篇 |
2012年 | 111篇 |
2011年 | 85篇 |
2010年 | 53篇 |
2009年 | 76篇 |
2008年 | 97篇 |
2007年 | 87篇 |
2006年 | 68篇 |
2005年 | 72篇 |
2004年 | 72篇 |
2003年 | 57篇 |
2002年 | 47篇 |
2001年 | 30篇 |
2000年 | 27篇 |
1999年 | 27篇 |
1998年 | 20篇 |
1997年 | 21篇 |
1996年 | 17篇 |
1995年 | 19篇 |
1994年 | 14篇 |
1993年 | 23篇 |
1992年 | 22篇 |
1991年 | 12篇 |
1990年 | 11篇 |
1989年 | 20篇 |
1988年 | 11篇 |
1987年 | 10篇 |
1986年 | 12篇 |
1985年 | 23篇 |
1984年 | 9篇 |
1983年 | 12篇 |
1982年 | 9篇 |
1981年 | 7篇 |
1980年 | 9篇 |
1979年 | 5篇 |
1978年 | 6篇 |
1977年 | 8篇 |
1975年 | 10篇 |
1972年 | 4篇 |
排序方式: 共有1939条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
41.
Stephanie K. V. Peschel;Sophia Fürtjes;Christine Sigrist;Catharina Voss;Johanna Berwanger;Theresa M. Ollmann;Hanna Kische;Frank Rückert;Julian Koenig;Lars Pieper;Katja Beesdo-Baum; 《Scandinavian journal of psychology》2024,65(2):252-263
Experiences of tension and difficulties in emotion regulation have been linked to eating pathology in clinical samples and are targeted in respective treatment approaches. The present study aimed to investigate the impact of tension on engagement in disordered eating behaviors (DEBs) and potential moderating effects of trait emotion regulation in young people from the general population. A subsample of 971 adolescents and young adults from an epidemiological cohort study reported on levels of tension and four different DEBs (skipping eating, restrained eating, eating large amounts of food, loss-of-control-eating) via ecological momentary assessment (EMA), as well as on trait emotion regulation via questionnaire. In multilevel models, momentary tension did not predict levels of subsequent DEBs. However, higher average levels of tension across the EMA period predicted higher levels of all DEBs. No interactions with emotion regulation emerged. Individuals experiencing overall higher levels of tension appear to be more prone to engaging in DEBs. 相似文献
42.
43.
44.
45.
Roncesvalles MN Schmitz C Zedka M Assaiante C Woollacott M 《Journal of motor behavior》2005,37(5):404-416
The authors investigated the emergence of independent control of body segments in bimanual tasks involving either voluntary or involuntary trunk motion by tracking the transition from an ego- to an exocentric mode of postural control during childhood (i.e., from body-referenced orientation to externally referenced action). A paradigm combining a seated manual task and various trunk manipulations described the coordination strategies used by 24 children at different ages (2 to 9 years) and by adults. The following questions were asked: (a) When do children begin to dissociate upper limb movements from those of the trunk? (b) What segmental strategies are exhibited by each age group (2-3, 4-6, and 7-9 years, and adults)? Kinematic analyses revealed that younger children (2-6 years) used either the trunk or the support surface as reference to orient the limbs. Older children (7-9 years) began to use a gravitational reference frame similar to that of adults; they uncoupled upper limb motion from the trunk in either voluntary or imposed conditions. Young children patterned the forearm trajectory after the initiating segment (support surface or the trunk), thus reducing the degrees of freedom during the dual task. Echoing previous reports, 7-9 years of age appears to be a critical period in which children master postural control and develop an internal representation of body scheme. 相似文献
46.
Two personality models are compared regarding their relationship with personality disorder (PD) symptom counts and with lifetime Axis I diagnoses. These models share 5 similar domains, and the Big 7 model also includes 2 domains assessing self-evaluation: positive and negative valence. The Big 7 model accounted for more variance in PDs than the 5-factor model, primarily because of the association of negative valence with most PDs. Although low-positive valence was associated with most Axis I diagnoses, the 5-factor model generally accounted for more variance in Axis I diagnoses than the Big 7 model. Some predicted associations between self-evaluation and psychopathology were not found, and unanticipated associations emerged. These findings are discussed regarding the utility of evaluative terms in clinical assessment. 相似文献
47.
After reading a murder or theft vignette in which the perpetrator was a 20-, 40-, or 60-year-old man, 95 undergraduates gave sentence and parole recommendations. Punishment was harsher for the murder than for the theft. For murder, participants treated the 20- and 60-year-old men less harshly than the 40-year-old man, which confirms previous archival findings. However, this inverted U-shaped function occurred for murder only. The authors discussed the results in the context of the just-desert and utilitarian rationales that guide sentencing and gave suggestions for future research. 相似文献
48.
Johansson B Hofer SM Allaire JC Maldonado-Molina MM Piccinin AM Berg S Pedersen NL McClearn GE 《Psychology and aging》2004,19(1):145-156
Change in cognitive abilities was assessed over a 6-year period in a sample of monozygotic and same-sex dizygotic twin pairs (N = 507 individuals), aged 80 and older (mean age = 83.3 years: SD = 3.1). who remained nondemented over the course of the study. Latent growth models (LGMs) show that chronological age and time to death are consistent predictors of decline in measures of memory, reasoning, speed, and verbal abilities. Multivariate LGM analysis resulted in weak and often negative correlations among rates of change between individuals within twin pairs, indicating greater differential change within twin pairs than occurs on average across twin pairs. These findings highlight several challenges for estimating genetic sources of variance in the context of compromised health and mortality-related change. 相似文献
49.
The nature and treatment of stuttering as revealed by fMRI A within- and between-group comparison 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
Neumann K Euler HA von Gudenberg AW Giraud AL Lanfermann H Gall V Preibisch C 《Journal of Fluency Disorders》2003,28(4):381-410
This article reviews some of our recent functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies of stuttering. Using event-related fMRI experiments, we investigated brain activation during speech production. Results of three studies comparing persons who stutter (PWS) and persons who do not stutter (PWNS) are outlined. Their findings point to a region in the right frontal operculum (RFO) that was consistently implicated in stuttering. During overt reading and before fluency shaping therapy, PWS showed higher and more distributed neuronal activation than PWNS. Immediately after therapy differential activations were even more distributed and left sided. They extended to frontal, temporal, and parietal regions, anterior cingulate, insula, and putamen. These over-activations were slightly reduced and again more right sided two years after therapy. Left frontal deactivations remained stable over two years of observation, and therefore possibly indicate a dysfunction. After therapy, we noted higher activations in persons who stutter moderately than in those who stutter severely. These activations might reflect patterns of compensation. We discuss why these findings suggest that fluency-inducing techniques might synchronize a disturbed signal transmission between auditory, speech motor planning, and motor areas.
Educational objectives: The reader will learn about and be able to: (1) identify regions of brain activations and deactivations specific for PWS; (2) describe brain activation changes induced by fluency shaping therapy; and (3) discuss the correlation between stuttering severity and brain activation. 相似文献
50.
The central issue addressed is whether there are electrophysiological markers for high-language proficiency in adults. Event-related brain potentials (ERPs) were recorded in 19 young adults who demonstrated either "normal" or "high" language proficiency. ERPs were obtained during a sentence-processing task and analyzed for specific response components elicited by different word types. The ERPs in the normal and high-language proficiency groups were similar for early sensory related processing but differed in the later components thought to be more closely related to lexical access and post-lexical processing. The findings for the ERP waveforms suggest that adults with high-language proficiency show slightly faster lexical access over left anterior brain regions for closed class items, and a reduced reliance on contextual information contained in sentences. 相似文献