首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   875篇
  免费   65篇
  国内免费   1篇
  941篇
  2023年   16篇
  2022年   9篇
  2021年   21篇
  2020年   27篇
  2019年   32篇
  2018年   51篇
  2017年   53篇
  2016年   46篇
  2015年   34篇
  2014年   43篇
  2013年   101篇
  2012年   62篇
  2011年   62篇
  2010年   35篇
  2009年   13篇
  2008年   45篇
  2007年   28篇
  2006年   26篇
  2005年   31篇
  2004年   24篇
  2003年   26篇
  2002年   32篇
  2001年   15篇
  2000年   8篇
  1999年   11篇
  1998年   9篇
  1997年   12篇
  1996年   7篇
  1995年   6篇
  1994年   10篇
  1993年   3篇
  1992年   3篇
  1991年   3篇
  1990年   2篇
  1989年   2篇
  1988年   2篇
  1987年   1篇
  1985年   3篇
  1984年   2篇
  1983年   6篇
  1982年   3篇
  1981年   3篇
  1979年   2篇
  1978年   1篇
  1976年   1篇
  1971年   4篇
  1966年   1篇
  1960年   1篇
  1958年   1篇
  1957年   1篇
排序方式: 共有941条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
921.
Background and Objectives: When couples face a stressful life event, this can adversely impact relationship satisfaction. Because savoring positive experiences is thought to enhance intimate relationships and there is evidence that savoring buffers the negative effects of stress at the intrapersonal level, this study examined savoring as an interpersonal resource for couples who experienced a stressful life event.

Methods: One hundred and twenty-eight opposite-sex couples completed measures of impact of event, savoring, positive affect, and relationship satisfaction.

Results: Results from actor-partner interdependence models found that: (1) For couple members who reported a relatively low impact of event, their own savoring was positively predicted by their partner’s impact of event; (2) For women, their own savoring the moment predicted their own and their partner’s greater relationship satisfaction; (3) The relationship between one’s own impact of event and relationship satisfaction was buffered by one’s partner’s savoring the moment; and (4) The relationship between one’s own savoring and relationship satisfaction was mediated by one’s own positive affect.

Conclusions: The findings support the study of savoring as an interpersonal resource in times of stress and have implications for couples-based interventions.  相似文献   

922.
Word fragment completion performance was examined for items that were presented in the same or different letter case at study and test. During the study phase words and nonwords were presented at central fixation, then during the test phase a divided visual field technique was used in which word fragments were presented briefly to the right hemisphere (left visual field) or the left hemisphere (right visual field). Previous research using the word stem completion task indicated that only the right hemisphere was sensitive to case changes in words from study to test. In contrast, the current results indicate that in the fragment completion task the priming effects for the test items presented to either hemisphere were greater when the fragments were in the same compared to different letter case at study and test. These results indicate that both hemispheres are capable of supporting form-specific visual implicit memory.  相似文献   
923.
924.
Guided by the theory of planned behavior, this 2-week longitudinal study examined health behaviors in a sample of 279 adolescents. Social norms and perceived behavioral control (PBC) were tested as predictors of self-reported intentions and behaviors in 2 domains, eating and physical activity. Differentiating, as opposed to aggregating, parent and peer norms provided unique information. For PBC, the authors distinguished global causality beliefs from self-related agency beliefs and intraself (effort, ability) from extraself (parents, teachers) means. Intraself agency beliefs strongly predicted healthy intentions, whereas intraself causality beliefs had a negative influence. Patterns differed somewhat across behaviors and gender. Results highlight theoretical issues and provide potential targets for research on health promotion programs for youth.  相似文献   
925.
Two studies investigated the interactive effects of trainees’ dispositional implicit theory of ability (entity versus incremental) and the use of a promotion versus prevention orientation during the cognitive modeling component of training in the use of a problem‐solving technique. The dependent variables were trainees’ self‐reported behavioral intentions (Study 1) and performance (Study 2), which was assessed by two independent judges following the training program and in a 1‐month follow‐up in the use of the problem‐solving technique. As predicted, entity theorists were more responsive to the social cues contained in the promotion and prevention orientations during the cognitive modeling of the problem‐solving technique. As a result, a promotion orientation produced stronger behavioral intentions (Study 1) and performance (Study 2) than a prevention orientation for entity theorists but not for incremental theorists. The results point to the need to understand the implicit theories of trainees, particularly those who have an entity theory of ability, and for trainers to adopt a strong promotion orientation in their presentation of materials. Trainers who stress the avoidance of errors and overemphasise the importance of performance may unwittingly undermine the intentions and performance of those who have doubts about the efficacy of the training. Deux recherches ont porté sur l’interaction chez des sujets en formation des conséquences de la théorie implicite des capacités dont chacun dispose (stables ou amendables) et de l’orientation vers la prudence ou le progrès lors de la modélisation cognitive dans l’utilisation d’une technique de résolution de problèmes. Les variables dépendantes étaient les intentions comportementales rapportées par les stagiaires (Recherche n? 1) et la performance (Recherche n° 2) évaluée par deux juges indépendants à la fin du programme de formation et un mois plus tard. Comme prévu, les partisans des capacités stables étaient plus sensibles aux signes sociaux présents dans les orientations prudence ou progrès lors de l’élaboration cognitive de la technique de résolution de problèmes. Une orientation vers le progrès était à l’origine d’intentions comportementales plus affirmées (Recherche n? 1) et d’une meilleure performance (Recherche n? 2) pour les tenants de la stabilité des capacités, mais pas pour ceux qui pensaient que les capacités pouvaient être améliorées. Ces résultats montrent qu’il est indispensable de prendre en considération les théories implicites des stagiaires, et plus particulièrement de ceux qui pensent que les capacités sont figées. Quant aux formateurs, il leur faut adopter une orientation fortement axée sur le progrès dans leur présentation des contenus. Les formateurs qui insistent sur l’évitement des erreurs et exagèrent l’importance de la performance peuvent involontairement nuire aux intentions et aux résultats de ceux qui doutent de l’efficacité de la formation.  相似文献   
926.
927.
928.
This article investigated the impact of breast cancer (BC) in middle-aged Australian women (45-50 years). Two waves of data collected 2 years apart from a longitudinal survey of 12,177 women identified 3 groups: (a) 11,933 (98%) who reported never having had BC, (b) 181 (1.5%) who reported a diagnosis of BC at Time 1, and (c) 63 (0.5%) who reported onset of BC between Time 1 and Time 2. Repeated measures analysis of variance was used to compare the 3 groups. Women with recent onset of BC experienced significant changes across a range of functioning compared with the other 2 groups. Compared with women with no BC, women with longer established onset of BC had significantly worse health and social outcomes, but these were associated with small effect sizes. Both groups of women with BC reported less impact on mental and emotional health than on other areas of functioning.  相似文献   
929.
930.
Abstract

Pre-school children find it difficult to correctly report if it is morning or afternoon. The present study tested whether children could learn a non-verbal Time-Place Learning (TPL) task that depended on time of day. Twenty-five 4-year-olds were repeatedly asked to find a toy in one of two boxes. Children in the Cued condition were told the toy was in one box in the morning and in another box in the afternoon. Children in the Not Cued condition were told the toy was sometimes in one box and sometimes in the other box. After 80 trials, children were asked if it was morning or afternoon. About 65% of the children learned the TPL task, and about three-quarters of the children verbally identified if it was morning or afternoon. However, the children who learned the TPL task were not necessarily the children who correctly answered whether it was morning or afternoon, and those in the Cued condition were no more likely to solve the task than those in the Not Cued condition. The implication is that children have a sense of time that can be used to solve spatio-temporal contingencies, but does not depend on the verbal understanding of time of day.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号