全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2308篇 |
免费 | 177篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
2487篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 31篇 |
2022年 | 25篇 |
2021年 | 47篇 |
2020年 | 83篇 |
2019年 | 69篇 |
2018年 | 123篇 |
2017年 | 112篇 |
2016年 | 119篇 |
2015年 | 96篇 |
2014年 | 91篇 |
2013年 | 329篇 |
2012年 | 137篇 |
2011年 | 146篇 |
2010年 | 95篇 |
2009年 | 73篇 |
2008年 | 141篇 |
2007年 | 102篇 |
2006年 | 77篇 |
2005年 | 69篇 |
2004年 | 53篇 |
2003年 | 70篇 |
2002年 | 69篇 |
2001年 | 54篇 |
2000年 | 29篇 |
1999年 | 27篇 |
1998年 | 20篇 |
1997年 | 21篇 |
1996年 | 20篇 |
1995年 | 8篇 |
1994年 | 11篇 |
1993年 | 8篇 |
1991年 | 9篇 |
1990年 | 7篇 |
1989年 | 5篇 |
1988年 | 7篇 |
1985年 | 9篇 |
1984年 | 5篇 |
1983年 | 9篇 |
1982年 | 9篇 |
1981年 | 5篇 |
1980年 | 3篇 |
1979年 | 4篇 |
1978年 | 6篇 |
1976年 | 3篇 |
1975年 | 7篇 |
1973年 | 6篇 |
1972年 | 3篇 |
1971年 | 6篇 |
1938年 | 3篇 |
1937年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有2487条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
81.
Contact with bisexual individuals reduces binegativity among heterosexuals and lesbian women and gay men 下载免费PDF全文
Ashley Lytle Christina Dyar Sheri R. Levy Bonita London 《European journal of social psychology》2017,47(5):580-599
Although binegativity, the stigmatization of bisexuality, is prevalent among heterosexual and lesbian and gay communities, little research has addressed how the quantity and quality of intergroup contact affect binegativity. Drawing on intergroup contact theory, this study examined contact with and attitudes toward bisexuals among heterosexual undergraduates, heterosexual adults, and lesbians and gay men. Knowing more bisexuals (quantity) predicted more positive attitudes toward and decreased intergroup anxiety with male and female bisexuals. A multilevel structural equation model indicated that contact quality simultaneously predicted higher perceived stability of female bisexuality (but not male bisexuality) and tolerance of and less intergroup anxiety with male and female bisexuals when contact quantity was controlled for. This research suggests that both quantity and quality of contact with bisexuals predicts improved intergroup attitudes. Implications for future research on reducing binegativity are discussed. 相似文献
82.
The Experience in Personal Social Systems Questionnaire (EXIS.pers): Development and Psychometric Properties 下载免费PDF全文
Christina Hunger Annette Bornhäuser Leoni Link Julian Geigges Andreas Voss Jan Weinhold Jochen Schweitzer 《Family process》2017,56(1):154-170
This study presents the theoretical background, development, and psychometric properties of the German and English versions of the Experience in Personal Social Systems Questionnaire (EXIS.pers). It assesses how the members of a personal social system experience their situation within that system. It is designed as a research tool for interventions in which only one member of the system participates (e.g., Family Constellation Seminars). The EXIS.pers was created to measure change on the individual level relating to one's own important personal social system. In Study 1, we used exploratory factor analysis (EFA) for latent variable identification of the original German EXIS.pers (n = 179). In Studies 2 and 3, we used confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) to examine the dimensionality of the German (n = 634) and English (n = 310) EXIS.pers. Internal consistencies and cross‐cultural structural equivalence were assessed. EFA indicated that a four‐factor model provided best fit for the German EXIS.pers. For both the German and English EXIS.pers, CFA provided the best fit for a five‐factor bi‐level model that included a general factor (Experience In Personal Social Systems) and four dimensions (Belonging, Autonomy, Accord, Confidence). Good internal consistencies, external associations, and cross‐cultural structural equivalence were demonstrated. This study provides first evidence for the German and English EXIS.pers as an economical and reliable measure of an individual's experience within his or her personal social systems. 相似文献
83.
James A. Naifeh PhD Matthew K. Nock PhD Robert J. Ursano MD Patti L. Vegella MS Pablo A. Aliaga MS Carol S. Fullerton PhD Ronald C. Kessler PhD Christina L. Wryter BA Steven G. Heeringa PhD Murray B. Stein MD MPH the Army STARRS Collaborators 《Suicide & life-threatening behavior》2017,47(5):589-602
This prospective cohort study used administrative data from the Army Study to Assess Risk and Resilience in Servicemembers to examine associations between neurocognitive functioning and subsequent suicidal events among Regular Army enlisted soldiers during the years 2004–2009. Cases were all soldiers who completed the Army's Automated Neuropsychological Assessment Metrics (ANAM) computerized testing battery prior to documented suicide attempt (n = 607), ideation (n = 955), or death (n = 57). Controls were an equal‐probability sample of 9,893 person‐months from other soldiers. Exploratory factor analysis of five ANAM tests identified a general neurocognitive factor that excluded the mathematic processing test (MTH). When examined separately in logistic regression analyses that controlled for sociodemographics and prior mental health diagnosis, both the general neurocognitive factor (logit [β] = ?.197 to ?.521; p < .01) and MTH (β = ?.024 to ?.064; p < .05) were associated with all outcomes. When both predictors were examined simultaneously, the general neurocognitive factor continued to be associated with all outcomes (β = ?.164 to ?.417; p < .05) and MTH continued to be associated with suicide attempt (β = ?.015; p = .046) and ideation (β = ?.014; p = .018). These small but robust associations suggest that future research must continue to examine the extent to which objective neurocognitive tests may enhance understanding and prediction of suicide risk. 相似文献
84.
Juhani Järvikivi Roger P. G. van Gompel Jukka Hyönä 《Journal of psycholinguistic research》2017,46(3):525-550
Two visual-world eye-tracking experiments investigating pronoun resolution in Finnish examined the time course of implicit causality information relative to both grammatical role and order-of-mention information. Experiment 1 showed an effect of implicit causality that appeared at the same time as the first-mention preference. Furthermore, when we counterbalanced the semantic roles of the verbs, we found no effect of grammatical role, suggesting the standard observed subject preference has a large semantic component. Experiment 2 showed that both the personal pronoun hän and the demonstrative tämä preferred the antecedent consistent with the implicit causality bias; tämä was not interpreted as referring to the semantically non-prominent entity. In contrast, structural prominence affected hän and tämä differently: we found a first-mention preference for hän, but a second-mention preference for tämä. The results suggest that semantic implicit causality information has an immediate effect on pronoun resolution and its use is not delayed relative to order-of-mention information. Furthermore, they show that order-of-mention differentially affects different types of anaphoric expressions, but semantic information has the same effect. 相似文献
85.
86.
Pollatsek A Hyönä J Bertram R 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》2000,26(2):820-833
The processing of transparent Finnish compound words was investigated in 2 experiments in which eye movements were recorded while sentences were read silently. The frequency of the second constituent had a large influence (95 ms) on gaze duration on the target words, but its influence was relatively late in processing: A clear effect only occurred on the probability of a third fixation. The frequency of the whole compound word had a similar influence on gaze duration (82 ms) and influenced eye movements at least as rapidly as did the frequency of the second constituent. These results, together with an earlier finding that the frequency of the first constituent affected the first fixation duration, indicate that the identification of these compound words involves parallel processing of both morphological constituents and whole-word representations. 相似文献
87.
88.
We measured driving performance (lane-keeping errors, driving times, and glances away from the road scene) in a video driving simulator for 24 volunteers who each drove alone on a 10.6-km multicurved course while simultaneously placing calls on a mobile phone subscribed to a voice-activated dialing system. Driving performance also was measured for the same distance while participants manually dialed phone numbers and while they drove without dialing. There were 22% fewer lane-keeping errors (p<.01) and 56% fewer glances away from the road scene (p<.01) when they used voice-activated dialing as compared to manual dialing. Significantly longer driving times in both of the dialing conditions as compared to the No Dialing condition are discussed in terms of the hypothesis that drivers decrease driving speed to compensate for the demands of the secondary phone tasks. 相似文献
89.
Cliffordson C 《Scandinavian journal of psychology》2002,43(1):49-59
The purpose of the present study was to examine the structure of empathy using a hierarchical approach, and to compare the dimensions of empathy with measures of social functioning, in order to contribute to the understanding of the nature of empathy. The dimensionality of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index, which comprises four subscales (empathic concern, perspective taking, fantasy and personal distress) was examined using confirmatory factor analysis. Relations with the Social Skills Inventory were also investigated. A sample of 127 applicants for places on nursing and social work undergraduate programs participated in the study. The study findings indicate that empathy is hierarchically organized, with one general dimension at the apex. The general factor is identical to empathic concern and this dimension overlaps to a great extent with perspective taking and fantasy. The findings also indicate that the general dimension constitutes an integrated entirety, with its main emphasis on emotional reactivity by also involving cognitive processes. 相似文献
90.
Kroll NE Yonelinas AP Dobbins IG Frederick CM 《Journal of experimental psychology. General》2002,131(2):241-254
A fundamental challenge to psychological research is the measurement of cognitive processes uncontaminated by response strategies resulting from different testing procedures. Test-free estimates of ability are vital when comparing the performance of different groups or different conditions. The current study applied several sets of measurement models to both forced-choice and yes-no recognition memory tests and concluded that the traditional signal-detection model resulted in distorted estimates of accuracy. Two-factor models were necessary to separate memory sensitivity from response bias. These models indicated that (a) memory accuracy did not differ across the tests and (b) the tests relied on the same underlying memory processes. The results illustrate the pitfalls of using a single-component model to measure accuracy in tasks that reflect 2 or more underlying processes. 相似文献