全文获取类型
收费全文 | 6069篇 |
免费 | 120篇 |
国内免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
6192篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 50篇 |
2023年 | 74篇 |
2022年 | 66篇 |
2021年 | 101篇 |
2020年 | 169篇 |
2019年 | 213篇 |
2018年 | 261篇 |
2017年 | 233篇 |
2016年 | 274篇 |
2015年 | 153篇 |
2014年 | 221篇 |
2013年 | 684篇 |
2012年 | 339篇 |
2011年 | 371篇 |
2010年 | 223篇 |
2009年 | 202篇 |
2008年 | 244篇 |
2007年 | 259篇 |
2006年 | 222篇 |
2005年 | 186篇 |
2004年 | 173篇 |
2003年 | 165篇 |
2002年 | 150篇 |
2001年 | 96篇 |
2000年 | 71篇 |
1999年 | 70篇 |
1998年 | 54篇 |
1997年 | 51篇 |
1996年 | 39篇 |
1995年 | 43篇 |
1994年 | 48篇 |
1993年 | 30篇 |
1992年 | 48篇 |
1991年 | 37篇 |
1990年 | 30篇 |
1989年 | 30篇 |
1988年 | 31篇 |
1987年 | 29篇 |
1986年 | 29篇 |
1985年 | 37篇 |
1984年 | 29篇 |
1983年 | 29篇 |
1982年 | 26篇 |
1980年 | 19篇 |
1979年 | 30篇 |
1978年 | 36篇 |
1977年 | 23篇 |
1975年 | 20篇 |
1974年 | 20篇 |
1973年 | 19篇 |
排序方式: 共有6192条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
951.
An experiment was performed to test whether cross-modal speaker matches could be made using isolated visible speech movement information. Visible speech movements were isolated using a point-light technique. In five conditions, subjects were asked to match a voice to one of two (unimodal) speaking point-light faces on the basis of speaker identity. Two of these conditions were designed to maintain the idiosyncratic speech dynamics of the speakers, whereas three of the conditions deleted or distorted the dynamics in various ways. Some of these conditions also equated video frames across dynamically correct and distorted movements. The results revealed generally better matching performance in the conditions that maintained the correct speech dynamics than in those conditions that did not, despite containing exactly the same video frames. The results suggest that visible speech movements themselves can support cross-modal speaker matching. 相似文献
952.
T. Ryan Byerly 《International Journal for Philosophy of Religion》2010,67(2):95-105
I argue that Alexander Pruss’s ontomystical arguments should not be endorsed without further argumentative support of their
premises. My specific targets are his claims that (i) Śamkara’s principle is true and (ii) the high mystics had phenomenal
experiences of radical dependence and as of a maximally great being. Against (i), I urge a host of counterexamples. The only
ways I can see for Pruss to respond to these counterexamples end up falsifying (ii). The key problem which leads to this conclusion
is that Pruss needs a criterion for distinguishing phenomenal experiences from non-phenomenal experiences according to which
the experiences of the high mystics were phenomenal experiences while the experiences of those persons I discuss in my counterexamples
to Śamkara’s principle are not. There appears to be no such criterion. I suggest that the future of the ontomystical arguments
lies in developing them as inductive rather than deductive arguments. 相似文献
953.
Michael W. Kraus Serena Chen Victoria A. Lee Laura D. Straus 《Journal of experimental social psychology》2010,46(6):1067-1073
Transference occurs when a perceiver's representation of a significant other is activated and applied to a new target person (Andersen & Chen, 2002). Extending past research, it was hypothesized that transference occurs even when a target person possesses a core feature—namely, group membership status—that is inapplicable to the relevant significant-other representation. Supporting this, transference led perceivers to make representation-consistent memory and evaluation judgments about a target—regardless of whether the target's political (experiment 1) or ethnic (experiment 2) group membership was the same or different from that of the relevant significant other. Moreover, in experiment 2, perceivers undergoing transference involving a positively evaluated significant other behaved more positively toward the target even when the target was from an ethnic out-group. The results represent initial evidence of transference processes occurring across group boundaries. Implications for transference and the reduction of out-group bias are discussed. 相似文献
954.
955.
ABSTRACT Dependency plays an important role in both normal and abnormal personality, but the construct remains loosely defined, both in clinical practice and the research literature. Moreover, although a number of measures purport to measure trait dependency, little agreement exists as to the structure of dependency within or across these instruments. Furthermore, what constitutes the low end of trait dependency remains unclear. Five hundred nine undergraduates completed a battery of 14 dependency scales and subscales, a subset of whom (n=322) also completed a broad measure of personality pathology (SNAP‐2). Exploratory factor analysis of the dependency measures yielded two correlated factors—Passive‐Submissive and Active‐Emotional dependency—as well as a third, unrelated factor, Detachment/Autonomy, all with differential correlates to the pathological personality traits of the SNAP‐2. Taken together, these data suggest a clear, 2‐factor model for trait dependency that is distinct from detachment/autonomy. 相似文献
956.
Marilyn Huckans Lee Hutson Elizabeth Twamley Amy Jak Jeffrey Kaye Daniel Storzbach 《Neuropsychology review》2013,23(1):63-80
To evaluate the efficacy of cognitive rehabilitation therapies (CRTs) for mild cognitive impairment (MCI). Our review revealed a need for evidence-based treatments for MCI and a lack of a theoretical rehabilitation model to guide the development and evaluation of these interventions. We have thus proposed a theoretical rehabilitation model of MCI that yields key intervention targets–cognitive compromise, functional compromise, neuropsychiatric symptoms, and modifiable risk and protective factors known to be associated with MCI and dementia. Our model additionally defines specific cognitive rehabilitation approaches that may directly or indirectly target key outcomes–restorative cognitive training, compensatory cognitive training, lifestyle interventions, and psychotherapeutic techniques. Fourteen randomized controlled trials met inclusion criteria and were reviewed. Studies markedly varied in terms of intervention approaches and selected outcome measures and were frequently hampered by design limitations. The bulk of the evidence suggested that CRTs can change targeted behaviors in individuals with MCI and that CRTs are associated with improvements in objective cognitive performance, but the pattern of effects on specific cognitive domains was inconsistent across studies. Other important outcomes (i.e., daily functioning, quality of life, neuropsychiatric symptom severity) were infrequently assessed across studies. Few studies evaluated long-term outcomes or the impact of CRTs on conversion rates from MCI to dementia or normal cognition. Overall, results from trials are promising but inconclusive. Additional well-designed and adequately powered trials are warranted and required before CRTs for MCI can be considered evidence-based. 相似文献
957.
David N. Lee 《Ecological Psychology》2013,25(3-4):221-250
Studies of sensory guidance of movement in animals show that large nervous systems are not necessary for accurate control, suggesting that guidance may be based on some simple principles. In search for those principles, a theory of guidance of movement is described, which has its roots in Gibson's pathfinding work on visual control of locomotion (J. J. Gibson, 1958/this issue). The theory is based on the use of the simple but powerful variable tau, the time-to-closure of a gap at the current gap closure rate (whatever the gap's dimension—distance, angle, force, etc.); and on the principle of tau-coupling (keeping two Ts in constant ratio). In this article, I show how tau-coupling could be used to synchronize movements and regulate their kinematics. Supportive experimental results are reported. I also show theoretically how sensory-taus, defined on sensory input arrays, can specify motion-taus through tau-coupling; how the braking procedure of keeping tau.dot stable is a particular case of tau.coupling; and how tools for steering (e.g., limbs, whole bodies, cars, or aircraft) could be built from tau-couplings, which would enable steering control in a variety of situations, including steering straight and curved courses to goals, steering and controlling speed at the same time, steering around obstacles, and asymptoting on surfaces as when landing. Some movements also involve intrinsic guidance from within, and a hypothesis on intrinsic guidance by tau is introduced, supported by experiments spanning different activities. 相似文献
958.
959.
Abstract The present paper provides a conceptual basis for the examination of mood in sport. The nature of the mood construct is discussed with reference to related constructs. A conceptual model is presented which proposes that certain mood dimensions interact to influence behavior. Depression is proposed to be the most important mood dimension, influencing the intensity of mood responses, the interrelationships among other mood dimensions, and moderating mood and performance relationships for anger and tension. It is hoped that the conceptual model will help guide applied interventions and will act as a catalyst for future research. 相似文献
960.
This study was an exploratory examination of the influence of mothers' teaching behaviors, strategies, and child-rearing attitudes on their children's ability to delay gratification. In an externally imposed delay of gratification situation, 30 mothers from a rural university community taught their children strategies that could help them refrain from touching a brightly wrapped present when the mothers left the room. Results showed that mothers of children who did not delay gratification exhibited teaching behaviors and child-rearing attitudes consistent with a permissive parenting style, whereas mothers of children who did delay gratification exhibited teaching behaviors and child-rearing attitudes consistent with an authoritative parenting style. The results of this study are discussed with respect to the development of children's self-control and self-regulatory abilities. 相似文献