全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2042篇 |
免费 | 27篇 |
专业分类
2069篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 9篇 |
2023年 | 17篇 |
2022年 | 15篇 |
2021年 | 20篇 |
2020年 | 42篇 |
2019年 | 47篇 |
2018年 | 53篇 |
2017年 | 73篇 |
2016年 | 75篇 |
2015年 | 60篇 |
2014年 | 79篇 |
2013年 | 240篇 |
2012年 | 110篇 |
2011年 | 121篇 |
2010年 | 75篇 |
2009年 | 78篇 |
2008年 | 95篇 |
2007年 | 111篇 |
2006年 | 67篇 |
2005年 | 77篇 |
2004年 | 62篇 |
2003年 | 43篇 |
2002年 | 47篇 |
2001年 | 26篇 |
2000年 | 22篇 |
1999年 | 30篇 |
1998年 | 23篇 |
1997年 | 10篇 |
1996年 | 18篇 |
1994年 | 15篇 |
1992年 | 17篇 |
1991年 | 10篇 |
1990年 | 15篇 |
1989年 | 19篇 |
1988年 | 13篇 |
1987年 | 9篇 |
1986年 | 13篇 |
1985年 | 21篇 |
1984年 | 10篇 |
1983年 | 18篇 |
1981年 | 7篇 |
1980年 | 14篇 |
1979年 | 16篇 |
1978年 | 8篇 |
1977年 | 7篇 |
1975年 | 7篇 |
1974年 | 11篇 |
1973年 | 9篇 |
1970年 | 7篇 |
1969年 | 7篇 |
排序方式: 共有2069条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
101.
Previous studies have shown that a change in an existing object is not as effective in capturing attention as the appearance
of a new object. This view was recently challenged by Lu and Zhou (Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 12:567–572, 2005), who
found strong capture effects for an object changing its color. We suspected that this finding critically depends on a procedural
particularity in Lu and Zhou’s study, namely that the color of the unique item and the color of the no-unique items randomly
switched between trials. In the current study we replicate Lu and Zhou’s capture effect (Experiment 1) and show that no capture
occurs when the color-to-stimuli assignment is fixed (Experiment 2). Two further experiments suggest that the capture effect
in Experiment 1 is not because the unique item switched color (Experiment 3), but because all the no-unique items switched
color (Experiment 4). The results are discussed considering top-down modulation and inter-trial priming effects.
相似文献
Adrian von Mühlenen (Corresponding author)Email: |
Markus ConciEmail: |
102.
We analyzed family dynamics and coping behaviors of parents with a child with an autistic spectrum disorder. Previous research
suggests that moderate levels of cohesion and adaptability are associated with higher levels of positive coping, and that
the more coping strategies a family implements, the greater their satisfaction with family functioning. Using a family systems
approach, the relationships among the familial variables of cohesion, adaptability, and social support were evaluated for
their contributions to coping in the family unit. We also compared the responses of mothers and fathers. Surprisingly, for
these parents of children with autism, those who rated their family as enmeshed implemented more positive coping strategies
than did those from other cohesion styles. This finding suggests that the enmeshed style may be more adaptive for a family
that encounters extreme challenges. Notably, mothers and fathers agreed on all variables except for perception of social support
from family and friends. Mothers perceived more social support from family and friends. The results lead to several interesting
suggestions for future research. 相似文献
103.
Chris J. Jackson 《Journal of research in personality》2011,45(1):29-36
This research determines if Mastery Goal Orientation mediates Sensation Seeking in the prediction of functional performance and if Sensation Seeking directly predicts dysfunctional behavior. Using two different measures of Sensation Seeking, a sample of fulltime Australian workers was used to test the proposed learning mechanism in the prediction of supervisor rated work outcomes, self-reported work outcomes, and self-reported dysfunctional behavior. As predicted, mediation and suppression analyses provided strong support for the proposed model but with just one of the measures of Sensation Seeking. It is concluded that this mechanism of learning has much to offer our understanding of functional and dysfunctional outcomes. 相似文献
104.
In this article we argue that self-deception evolved to facilitate interpersonal deception by allowing people to avoid the cues to conscious deception that might reveal deceptive intent. Self-deception has two additional advantages: It eliminates the costly cognitive load that is typically associated with deceiving, and it can minimize retribution if the deception is discovered. Beyond its role in specific acts of deception, self-deceptive self-enhancement also allows people to display more confidence than is warranted, which has a host of social advantages. The question then arises of how the self can be both deceiver and deceived. We propose that this is achieved through dissociations of mental processes, including conscious versus unconscious memories, conscious versus unconscious attitudes, and automatic versus controlled processes. Given the variety of methods for deceiving others, it should come as no surprise that self-deception manifests itself in a number of different psychological processes, and we discuss various types of self-deception. We then discuss the interpersonal versus intrapersonal nature of self-deception before considering the levels of consciousness at which the self can be deceived. Finally, we contrast our evolutionary approach to self-deception with current theories and debates in psychology and consider some of the costs associated with self-deception. 相似文献
105.
Langley LK Friesen CK Saville AL Ciernia AT 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》2011,73(5):1546-1561
This study examined adult age differences in reflexive orienting to two types of uninformative spatial cues: central arrows and peripheral onsets. In two experiments using a Posner cuing task, young adults (ages 18–28 years), young-old adults (60–74 years), and old-old adults (75–92 years) responded to targets that were preceded 100–1,000 ms earlier by a central arrow or a peripheral abrupt onset. In Experiment 1, the cue remained present upon target onset. Facilitation effects at short cue–target stimulus onset asynchronies (SOAs) were prolonged in duration for the two older groups relative to the young adults. At longer cue–target SOAs, inhibition of return (IOR) that was initiated by peripheral onset cues was observed in the performance of young adults but not in that of the two older groups. In Experiment 2, the cue was presented briefly and removed prior to target onset. The change in cue duration minimized age differences (particularly for young-old adults) in facilitation effects and led to IOR for all three age groups. The findings are consistent with the idea that attentional control settings change with age, with higher settings for older adults leading to delayed disengagement from spatial cues. 相似文献
106.
Tilmann von Soest Svein Mossige Kari Stefansen Odin Hjemdal 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》2010,32(2):215-225
In this study, the validity of the recently developed 28-item Resilience Scale for Adolescents (READ) was examined. Survey
data from a representative sample of 6,723 Norwegian senior high school students between 18 and 20 years of age were used
for this purpose. Validity and reliability were investigated by means of exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis, correlations
with other relevant variables, and internal consistency measures. The results supported the construct and convergent validity
of the five factors of a modified 23-item version of the scale, which also yielded acceptable psychometric properties. The
paper concludes that the modified READ is a valid measure assessing relevant resilience factors with relatively few items,
thereby serving as a valuable tool in resilience and risk-factor research. 相似文献
107.
Sophie von Stumm Sally Macintyre David G. Batty Heather Clark Ian J. Deary 《Intelligence》2010,38(1):202-211
In a birth cohort of 6281 men from Aberdeen, Scotland, social class of origin, childhood intelligence, childhood behavior disturbance and education were examined as predictors of status attainment in midlife (46 to 51 years). Social class of origin, intelligence and behavior disturbance were conceptualized as correlated predictors, whose effects were hypothesized to be partially mediated by educational qualifications. A structural equation model using Full Information Maximum Likelihood estimation confirmed that education had the strongest direct effect on status attainment at midlife. Furthermore, education partially mediated the effects of social class of origin and childhood intelligence, and fully mediated the effects of behavior disturbance on status attainment. Social class of origin, childhood intelligence and behavior disturbance were strongly inter-correlated. After controlling for these associations, educational and social status attainments were influenced to a considerably greater extent by childhood intelligence than by social class of origin. 相似文献
108.
W von Suchodoletz V Airapetjanz W Gierow 《Psychiatrie, Neurologie, und medizinische Psychologie》1985,37(12):713-717
The cross-correlation method was used to measure interhemisphere phase differences over four areas of the brain in 22 healthy children aged between 7 and 10 years. Apart from defining normal values, the authors describe age-dependent characteristics and changes that take place as a result of mental stress. 相似文献
109.
Alina A. von Davier 《Psychometrika》2013,78(4):605-623
In this paper, an overview of the observed-score equating (OSE) process is provided from the perspective of a unifying equating framework (von Davier in von Davier (Ed.), Statistical models for test equating, scaling, and linking, Springer, New York, pp. 1–17, 2011b). The framework includes all OSE approaches. Issues related to the test, common items, and sampling designs and their relationship to measurement and equating are discussed. Challenges to the equating process, model assumptions, and approaches to equating evaluation are also presented. The equating process is illustrated step-by-step with a real data example from a licensure test. 相似文献
110.
The central idea of the good lives model (GLM) is that new criminal offences can be prevented above all by virtue of a satisfying way of life in the wake of the criminal offence. One important rehabilitation strategy is to support clients in this way of life. The aim of the article is to provide a concise overview of the criteria that define this kind of life from the point of view of the GLM, to evaluate the relationship of criminal behavior with regard to these criteria and to point out ways how the rehabilitation model can be fruitfully applied in practical and therapeutic terms. The article closes with a critical evaluation of the rehabilitation model. 相似文献