首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   573篇
  免费   60篇
  国内免费   58篇
  2024年   2篇
  2023年   15篇
  2022年   7篇
  2021年   14篇
  2020年   14篇
  2019年   14篇
  2018年   20篇
  2017年   34篇
  2016年   18篇
  2015年   18篇
  2014年   20篇
  2013年   68篇
  2012年   29篇
  2011年   38篇
  2010年   35篇
  2009年   38篇
  2008年   37篇
  2007年   42篇
  2006年   28篇
  2005年   27篇
  2004年   30篇
  2003年   18篇
  2002年   17篇
  2001年   14篇
  2000年   13篇
  1999年   10篇
  1998年   5篇
  1997年   10篇
  1996年   3篇
  1995年   3篇
  1994年   2篇
  1993年   6篇
  1992年   4篇
  1991年   2篇
  1990年   4篇
  1989年   7篇
  1988年   1篇
  1987年   3篇
  1986年   3篇
  1985年   3篇
  1984年   1篇
  1983年   1篇
  1982年   2篇
  1980年   2篇
  1978年   1篇
  1973年   4篇
  1971年   1篇
  1969年   1篇
  1968年   1篇
  1964年   1篇
排序方式: 共有691条查询结果,搜索用时 562 毫秒
571.
影响当代中国人宗教信仰倾向的几个因素辨析   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
在我国特殊的社会文化背景下,性别、年龄、文化程度等常见自变量对民众宗教信仰倾向的影响呈现出复杂、多样的特征.本文以零点调查的全国抽样数据为基础,对上述问题进行了深入分析,揭示出重大社会事件这一显著影响因素,进而指出意识形态与泛神文化共同构成了民众宗教信仰倾向形成的重要背景.  相似文献   
572.
中小学教师工作-家庭冲突的测量   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2       下载免费PDF全文
本研究旨在分析我国中小学教师工作-家庭冲突的结构并编制适合我国中小学教师的工作-家庭冲突问卷。在文献分析基础上,通过开放式问卷调查、教师访谈,分析1025份问卷数据,确定了中小学教师工作-家庭冲突包括工作干扰家庭和家庭干扰工作两方面,两方面均包括情绪情感、心理资源和行为方式三维度。编制的我国中小学教师工作-家庭冲突问卷共22个项目,信效度良好,适合用来分析中小学教师工作-家庭冲突的状况。  相似文献   
573.
ABSTRACT Coping flexibility refers to the formulation of flexible coping strategies that meet distinct demands under changing circumstances. Dialectical thinking was proposed to be related to flexible coping across situations. The hypothesized link between dialectical thinking and coping flexibility was explored by a multimethod approach. In Study 1 , the association between dialectical thinking and coping flexibility was examined using a cross-sectional design. In Study 2 , the hypothesized link was tested using an experimental paradigm in which dialectical thinking was manipulated by priming procedures. Participants' responses to different hypothetical stressful situations were assessed after priming. Study 3 adopted a prospective design in which dialectical thinking assessed at an initial phase was a predictor of changes in coping flexibility and state anxiety over a 12-month period. Results from the three studies consistently revealed a positive relationship between dialectical thinking and coping flexibility.  相似文献   
574.
Self-reported or explicit self-esteem frequently conflicts with indirectly assessed, implicit self-esteem. The present research investigated whether meditation may reduce such inner conflicts by promoting congruence between implicit and explicit self-esteem. Relative to control conditions, meditation led to greater congruence between explicit self-esteem, assessed via self-report, and implicit self-esteem, indicated by name-letter preference (Studies 1 and 2). Low implicit self-esteem was further associated with a slow-down of explicit self-evaluation (Study 2), an effect that mediated the greater congruence between implicit and explicit self-esteem in the meditation condition. These results suggest that meditation encourages people to rely more on intuitive feelings of self-worth.  相似文献   
575.
576.
Background: this study sets out to examine to what extent attributional style (internal, stable, global) and self-esteem predicted positive affect and self-reported happiness in a normal, non-clinical, population of young people in their early 20s. Method: 88 participants completed five questionnaires: Attributional Style Questionnaire (ASQ in both positive and negative situations), Bradburn Affect Scales (Positive Affect, Negative Affect, and Affect Balance Scale), Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale Gurin Happiness Measure, and Oxford Happiness Inventory (OHI). Results: regressional analysis showed that the ASQ was the significant predictor of happiness accounting for 18% of variance. The ASQ in positive and negative situations was significantly inversely correlated with self-esteem. Further, with happiness (the OHI) as dependent variable and attributional style, self-esteem, and demographic variables as independent variables, self-esteem and attributional style (in positive situations) were the significant predictors of happiness accounting for 55% of the total variance. Conclusion: the results indicated that optimistic attributional style in positive situations was a stronger predictor of self-reported happiness than optimistic attributional style in negative situations. The implications of the findings are discussed in terms of cognitive strategies for changing pessimism into optimism thus enhancing happiness or psychological well-being.  相似文献   
577.
增龄性记忆改变规律及影响因素的研究   总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3  
郑虹  程灶火 《心理科学》2002,25(4):402-405
本研究用多维记忆量表对280名50~91岁的老年人做了测查,同时收集了一般资料,以探讨增龄性记忆改变的规律和年龄、教育和职业等因素对增龄性记忆改变的影响。结果表明:随年龄增长,各记忆组合分和分测验成绩均呈下降趋势,外显记忆成绩在70岁后呈快速下降,内隐记忆和日常生活记忆在75岁后才有明显下降;记忆总分、外显记忆和日常生活记忆均受年龄和教育的影响,内隐记忆不受教育的影响。结果提示老年人记忆随年龄增长呈渐渐下降,不同记忆功能开始下降的年龄和下降速率不同,教育对增龄性记忆改变有保护作用。  相似文献   
578.
How does diversity affect individuals and the groups in which they are embedded? This article examines this question using recent theory and research on Identity Integration (II). II refers to an individual's perceptions about whether two distinct social identities, or social groups to which individuals belong, are viewed as compatible (high II) or not (low II). A review of extant research suggests that individuals with high II are better at simultaneously accessing multiple identities and identity‐related knowledge and have improved well‐being and social outcomes. Expanding on this work, we argue that individuals who have higher II, and social collectives that foster II within their members, are more likely to reap the benefits of diversity.  相似文献   
579.
Animals relocating a target corner in a rectangular space often make rotational errors searching not only at the target corner but also at the diagonally opposite corner. The authors tested whether view-based navigation can explain rotational errors by recording panoramic snapshots at regularly spaced locations in a rectangular box. The authors calculated the global image difference between the image at each location and the image recorded at a target location in 1 of the corners, thus creating a 2-dimensional map of image differences. The authors found the most pronounced minima of image differences at the target corner and the diagonally opposite corner--conditions favoring rotational errors. The authors confirmed these results in virtual reality simulations and showed that the relative salience of different visual cues determines whether image differences are dominated by geometry or by features. The geometry of space is thus implicitly contained in panoramic images and does not require explicit computation by a dedicated module. A testable prediction is that animals making rotational errors in rectangular spaces are guided by remembered views.  相似文献   
580.
A concern for fairness is a fundamental and universal element of morality. To examine the extent to which cultural norms are integrated into fairness cognitions and influence social preferences regarding equality and equity, a large sample of children (N 2,163) aged 4–11 were tested in 13 diverse countries. Children participated in three versions of a third‐party, contextualized distributive justice game between two hypothetical recipients differing in terms of wealth, merit, and empathy. Social decision‐making in these games revealed universal age‐related shifts from equality‐based to equity‐based distribution motivations across cultures. However, differences in levels of individualism and collectivism between the 13 countries predicted the age and extent to which children favor equity in each condition. Children from the most individualistic cultures endorsed equitable distributions to a greater degree than children from more collectivist cultures when recipients differed in regards to wealth and merit. However, in an empathy context where recipients differed in injury, children from the most collectivist cultures exhibited greater preferences to distribute resource equitably compared to children from more individualistic cultures. Children from the more individualistic cultures also favored equitable distributions at an earlier age than children from more collectivist cultures overall. These results demonstrate aspects of both cross‐cultural similarity and divergence in the development of fairness preferences.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号