全文获取类型
收费全文 | 636篇 |
免费 | 63篇 |
国内免费 | 31篇 |
专业分类
730篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 9篇 |
2023年 | 18篇 |
2022年 | 9篇 |
2021年 | 16篇 |
2020年 | 16篇 |
2019年 | 14篇 |
2018年 | 20篇 |
2017年 | 37篇 |
2016年 | 20篇 |
2015年 | 20篇 |
2014年 | 21篇 |
2013年 | 73篇 |
2012年 | 29篇 |
2011年 | 38篇 |
2010年 | 35篇 |
2009年 | 38篇 |
2008年 | 37篇 |
2007年 | 44篇 |
2006年 | 28篇 |
2005年 | 30篇 |
2004年 | 31篇 |
2003年 | 19篇 |
2002年 | 18篇 |
2001年 | 14篇 |
2000年 | 13篇 |
1999年 | 10篇 |
1998年 | 5篇 |
1997年 | 11篇 |
1996年 | 3篇 |
1995年 | 3篇 |
1994年 | 2篇 |
1993年 | 6篇 |
1992年 | 4篇 |
1991年 | 2篇 |
1990年 | 4篇 |
1989年 | 7篇 |
1988年 | 2篇 |
1987年 | 3篇 |
1986年 | 3篇 |
1985年 | 3篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1983年 | 1篇 |
1982年 | 2篇 |
1980年 | 2篇 |
1978年 | 1篇 |
1973年 | 4篇 |
1971年 | 1篇 |
1969年 | 1篇 |
1968年 | 1篇 |
1964年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有730条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
281.
282.
The processing advantage of Subject-gapped relative clause (SRC) versus Object-gapped relative clause (ORC) has been advocated by competing processing accounts. Using a self-paced listening paradigm, this study investigates what Chinese RC online processing asymmetry looks like under concurrent memory load manipulation. Both On-line listening times and Post-online measures of Chinese SRCs and ORCs are estimated and compared. The on-line results show that ORCs and SRCs demonstrate no differential processing patterns under the interfering conditions. At the relativizer-DE marker region, under 0-digt-load, SRCs show processing advantage, while under 5-digit-load condition, SRCs display greater listening times than ORCs. Furthermore, the Post-online RTs and accuracy of post-sentence comprehension and digit recalls show that processing of SRCs had worse performance. These results lead to the conjecture that there may be no intrinsic processing asymmetry in Chinese RCs, and underscore the necessity that future studies in exploring the processing metrics of sentence complexity should consider the working memory involvement. 相似文献
283.
The purpose of this study was to investigate coming out to family and friends and their relationships to shame, internalized heterosexism, lesbian identity, and perceived social support in Chinese lesbians from 2 different cultural settings-Mainland China (N = 244) and Hong Kong (N = 234). Results of structural equation modeling showed that, in both samples, a sense of shame was related to internalized heterosexism and a devaluation of one's lesbian identity, which in turn was related to a decreased likelihood of coming out to others. Shame was also associated with a reduced perception of support from friends, which seemed in turn to exacerbate internalized heterosexism among lesbians. Family support was generally unrelated to outness, except for outness to friends in the Hong Kong sample. Results are discussed in relation to the cultural stigma attached to same-sex orientation and the cultural practice of shaming that parents use to socialize children. 相似文献
284.
[编者按] 黄俊杰教授是在海内外享有盛誉的台湾学者.现任台湾大学历史系特聘教授、中央研究院中国文哲研究所合聘研究员、台大人文社会高等研究院院长,并兼任台湾通识教育学会荣誉理事长. 相似文献
285.
古代汉语典籍中的法治语词略考 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
在中国古代的典籍中,因法、治二字的连属,出现了法的治之状态、作为名词的法治和以法治国的短语三种形态,表明中国古代已有法治这一名词,并有法治的概念. 相似文献
286.
287.
288.
从心所欲,不逾矩是孔子进入化境的重要文献依据,后人往往依此句来讨论儒家的自由观念.在后儒的理解和诠释中,从心所欲,不逾矩实际上是要求人们将社会礼法内化成自己的信念,并以此涤除不合礼法的欲望,使自己的欲念全在礼法当中,从而感觉不到礼法与内心要求的冲突.这种自由是不去挑战世俗礼法的心灵自在,它蕴含着牺牲自己内心来迎合不合理的社会规矩之危险.当然,它也意味着,在现实生活中,个体难以脱离具体的历史情境追求无所顾忌的个人自由. 相似文献
289.
The article presents a Bayesian model of causal learning that incorporates generic priors--systematic assumptions about abstract properties of a system of cause-effect relations. The proposed generic priors for causal learning favor sparse and strong (SS) causes--causes that are few in number and high in their individual powers to produce or prevent effects. The SS power model couples these generic priors with a causal generating function based on the assumption that unobservable causal influences on an effect operate independently (P. W. Cheng, 1997). The authors tested this and other Bayesian models, as well as leading nonnormative models, by fitting multiple data sets in which several parameters were varied parametrically across multiple types of judgments. The SS power model accounted for data concerning judgments of both causal strength and causal structure (whether a causal link exists). The model explains why human judgments of causal structure (relative to a Bayesian model lacking these generic priors) are influenced more by causal power and the base rate of the effect and less by sample size. Broader implications of the Bayesian framework for human learning are discussed. 相似文献
290.
Gordon L. Flett Paul L. Hewitt Wing Man Winnie Cheng 《Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy》2008,26(3):194-205
The current study examined the associations between dimensions of perfectionism and irrational beliefs in an adolescent sample.
In addition, we tested the association between psychological distress and irrational beliefs in adolescents and we evaluated
the feasibility of creating a modified version of the Survey of Personal Beliefs for use with adolescents. A sample of 250
adolescents (108 males, 142 females) completed the Child-Adolescent Perfectionism Scale, the Survey of Personal Beliefs, and
the CES-D Depression Scale. Initial psychometric analyses yielded an abbreviated 30-item Survey of Personal Beliefs with adequate
internal consistency for four of the five subscales. Our results showed that self-oriented perfectionism was associated significantly
with all five irrational belief subscales, while the associations between socially prescribed perfectionism and irrational
belief subscales were much smaller in magnitude. Self-oriented perfectionism, socially prescribed perfectionism, and irrational
beliefs were all associated significantly with elevated distress among adolescents, and irrational beliefs predicted unique
variance in distress, over and above the variance attributable to the trait perfectionism dimensions. The findings confirmed
the association between perfectionism and irrational beliefs and their respective roles in psychological distress among adolescents. 相似文献