首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   266篇
  免费   9篇
  2023年   4篇
  2022年   2篇
  2021年   4篇
  2020年   8篇
  2019年   8篇
  2018年   12篇
  2017年   8篇
  2016年   9篇
  2015年   5篇
  2014年   4篇
  2013年   50篇
  2012年   16篇
  2011年   13篇
  2010年   5篇
  2009年   8篇
  2008年   16篇
  2007年   15篇
  2006年   10篇
  2005年   6篇
  2004年   11篇
  2003年   3篇
  2002年   7篇
  2001年   3篇
  2000年   3篇
  1998年   2篇
  1997年   3篇
  1996年   2篇
  1994年   2篇
  1992年   1篇
  1991年   1篇
  1989年   2篇
  1988年   2篇
  1987年   2篇
  1985年   3篇
  1984年   2篇
  1983年   1篇
  1982年   1篇
  1978年   1篇
  1974年   2篇
  1973年   2篇
  1971年   2篇
  1970年   1篇
  1969年   2篇
  1968年   1篇
  1967年   1篇
  1965年   1篇
  1960年   1篇
  1959年   1篇
  1958年   2篇
  1957年   2篇
排序方式: 共有275条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
211.
Motor patterns in normal human gait are evident in several biomechanical and EMG analyses over the stride period. Some of these patterns are invariant over the stride period with changes of cadence, while others are closely correlated with speed changes. The findings for slow, natural, and fast walking are summarized: 1. Joint angle patterns over the stride period are quite invariant, and do not change with cadence;

2. Moment of force patterns at the ankle are least variable and quite consistent at all speeds;

3. A recently defined support moment is quite consistent at all speeds.

4. Moments at the knee and hip are highly variable at all cadences but decrease their variability as cadence increases;

5. Mechanical power patterns at all joints show consistent timing over the stride period;

6. EMG profiles of 5 muscles show consistent timing over the stride, but the amplitude increases as walking speed increases.

Arguments are presented to support the concept that walking speed is largely controlled by gain and that the timing of the motor patterns, which is extremely tightly synchronized with the anatomical position, is under major afferent control.  相似文献   
212.
213.
One hundred twenty-six three-member families were asked to produce conjointly three TAT stories based on nine cards, which were scored by the Arnold system of Story Sequence Analysis. There were 50 families with normal children, 44 with emotionally maladjusted, 16 with schizophrenic, and 16 with delinquent children. Analysis of the results revealed that the Arnold score successfully differentiated normal from abnormal families, but that the three abnormal groups did not differ from each other. The last set of TAT cards given was the most effective.  相似文献   
214.
The current study examines the degree to which individual differences in cognitive ability and cognitive style (rational thinking vs. experiential thinking) uniquely and jointly account for differences in religiosity. Using an array of measures of religiosity, results show that cognitive ability has a medium to large negative effect on various aspects of religiosity. Though also negatively related to religiosity, rational thinking style did not add significant unique effects, nor did it convey a significant indirect effect from cognitive ability. Experiential thinking was generally unrelated to ability but was positively related to some aspects of religiosity. Overall the results confirm that those with higher cognitive ability are less likely to accept religious doctrine or engage in religious behaviors and those with lower ability are more likely to accept religious doctrine and exhibit higher levels of fundamentalism. Cognitive style appears to play a lesser role in explaining individual differences in religiosity than cognitive ability.  相似文献   
215.
Exploratory factor analysis (EFA) is generally regarded as a technique for large sample sizes (N), with N = 50 as a reasonable absolute minimum. This study offers a comprehensive overview of the conditions in which EFA can yield good quality results for N below 50. Simulations were carried out to estimate the minimum required N for different levels of loadings (λ), number of factors (f), and number of variables (p) and to examine the extent to which a small N solution can sustain the presence of small distortions such as interfactor correlations, model error, secondary loadings, unequal loadings, and unequal p/f. Factor recovery was assessed in terms of pattern congruence coefficients, factor score correlations, Heywood cases, and the gap size between eigenvalues. A subsampling study was also conducted on a psychological dataset of individuals who filled in a Big Five Inventory via the Internet. Results showed that when data are well conditioned (i.e., high λ, low f, high p), EFA can yield reliable results for N well below 50, even in the presence of small distortions. Such conditions may be uncommon but should certainly not be ruled out in behavioral research data.  相似文献   
216.
The repeal of the Don't Ask, Don't Tell policy raises several questions and this article examines two of these: (1) Do gay/lesbian service members elicit personal discomfort in coworkers? and (2) Do gay/lesbian service members receive equitable administrative actions? Active duty Air Force office personnel (N?=?181) reported their attitudes toward open service and responded to hypothetical scenarios depicting gay/lesbian or heterosexual male/female subordinates. Gays/lesbians elicited more personal comfort than heterosexuals, and no differences existed between gays/lesbians and heterosexuals in administrative actions. Attitudes toward open service predicted personal comfort but not administrative action ratings.  相似文献   
217.
There are only a few studies focusing on the mental structure and its change in children and adolescents with psychiatric disorders. This study aimed at investigating change in the mental structure of children and adolescents over time. At intake 60 inpatients were interviewed with the operationalized psychodynamic diagnostic interview for children and adolescents (OPD-CA). After an average of 27 months 25 patients agreed on participating in the follow-up study. The OPD-CA was conducted again and the Kiddie-Sads present and lifetime version (Kiddie-Sads-PL) as well as parent-based ratings child behaviour checklist (CBCL) and strength and difficulties questionnaire (SDQ) were applied. A consensus rating of the OPD-CA was attained by three researchers well experienced with the use of the OPD-CA. The level of structural integration improved significantly, in particular in the dimensions “control” and “communication skills”. There were no significant changes in the dimension “self-perception/object-perception”. The improvement was also observable in parent-report questionnaires. This study makes a contribution to the idea that changes in the mental structure of children and adolescents can be shown and measured with the OPD-CA.  相似文献   
218.
This article is one of a series of personal construct investigations of the aftermath of the brutal civil war in Sierra Leone. It is based on interviews conducted with nine members of the Sierra Leone Single-Leg Amputee Football Club, all of whom lost a limb during the war. Interview themes concerned participants’ construing of this event and its perpetrators, the constriction of their lives and the emotions they subsequently experienced, the choices that they made, and the role of football and of forgiveness in their recovery and in posttraumatic growth.  相似文献   
219.
Facial caricatures exaggerate the distinctive features of a face and may elevate the recognition of a familiar face. We investigate whether the recognition of facial composites, or pictures of criminal faces, could be similarly enhanced. In this study, participants first estimated the degree of caricature necessary to make composites most identifiable. Contrary to expectation, an anticaricature was found to be best, presumably as this tended to reduce the appearance of errors. In support of this explanation, more positive caricature estimates were assigned to morphed composites: representations that tend to contain less overall error. In addition, anticaricaturing reduced identification for morphed composites but enhanced identification for individual composites. Although such improvements were too small to be of value to law enforcement, a sizeable naming benefit was observed when presenting a range of caricature states, which appeared to capitalize on individual differences in the internal representation of familiar faces.  相似文献   
220.
We live in a world in which scientific expertise and its epistemic authority become more important. On the other hand, the financial interests in research, which could potentially corrupt science, are increasing. Due to these two tendencies, a concern for the integrity of scientific research becomes increasingly vital. This concern is, however, hollow if we do not have a clear account of research integrity. Therefore, it is important that we explicate this concept. Following Rudolf Carnap’s characterization of the task of explication, this means that we should develop a concept that is (1) similar to our common sense notion of research integrity, (2) exact, (3) fruitful, and (4) as simple as possible. Since existing concepts do not meet these four requirements, we develop a new concept in this article. We describe a concept of epistemic integrity that is based on the property of deceptiveness, and argue that this concept does meet Carnap’s four requirements of explication. To illustrate and support our claims we use several examples from scientific practice, mainly from biomedical research.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号