全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3575篇 |
免费 | 62篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
3638篇 |
出版年
2019年 | 46篇 |
2018年 | 54篇 |
2017年 | 53篇 |
2016年 | 70篇 |
2015年 | 47篇 |
2014年 | 53篇 |
2013年 | 358篇 |
2012年 | 99篇 |
2011年 | 112篇 |
2010年 | 75篇 |
2009年 | 73篇 |
2008年 | 132篇 |
2007年 | 103篇 |
2006年 | 105篇 |
2005年 | 108篇 |
2004年 | 88篇 |
2003年 | 109篇 |
2002年 | 116篇 |
2001年 | 56篇 |
2000年 | 46篇 |
1999年 | 55篇 |
1998年 | 39篇 |
1997年 | 45篇 |
1996年 | 43篇 |
1995年 | 38篇 |
1994年 | 47篇 |
1993年 | 57篇 |
1992年 | 39篇 |
1991年 | 58篇 |
1990年 | 43篇 |
1989年 | 31篇 |
1988年 | 41篇 |
1987年 | 44篇 |
1986年 | 37篇 |
1985年 | 44篇 |
1984年 | 66篇 |
1983年 | 49篇 |
1982年 | 57篇 |
1981年 | 58篇 |
1980年 | 42篇 |
1979年 | 50篇 |
1978年 | 43篇 |
1977年 | 58篇 |
1976年 | 57篇 |
1975年 | 51篇 |
1974年 | 54篇 |
1973年 | 57篇 |
1972年 | 28篇 |
1967年 | 28篇 |
1966年 | 39篇 |
排序方式: 共有3638条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
171.
Charles M 《American journal of psychoanalysis》2004,64(3):305-19; discussion 301-4
This paper explores the issue of how character is created and re-created in the context of relationships. This theme, salient in the recent film The Hours, has been particularly problematic for creative women, who are often caught in tensions between self-development and relationship. Two case examples are given, in counterpoint to the film and to illustrations from Woolf's life and work. Through these various lenses, we can consider the complex interplays between our conjectures as to the expected price of relationship, and the actual price exacted as our various dramas unfold. 相似文献
172.
A 16-element movement sequence was taught under part-whole and whole-practice conditions. Participants (N = 18) produced a right-arm lever movement to sequentially presented target locations. The authors constructed part-whole practice by providing practice on only the 1st 8 elements on the 1st day of practice (100 repetitions of the 8-element sequence) and on all 16 elements on the 2nd day of practice (100 repetitions of the 16-element sequence). The whole-practice group practiced all 16 elements on both days (100 repetitions of the 16-element sequence per day). No differences in sequence structure or in movement duration of the 16-element sequence were noted on the retention test (Day 3). On transfer tests in which the 1st and last 8 elements were tested separately, however, the participants in the part-whole practice group performed more quickly than the participants in the whole-practice group, especially on the last 8 elements. Participants in the whole-practice group appeared to code the sequence so that it was relatively difficult to fully partition it into separate movements. Thus, on the transfer tests, there continued to be residual effects of the 8 elements that did not have to be produced but slowed down the rate of responding for the whole-practice group. That finding was not observed for the part-whole practice group. 相似文献
173.
Twelve patients with Alzheimer's disease (AD) and 15 healthy elderly control subjects were shown sets of luminance-defined letters, texture-defined letters, luminance-defined squares, and texture-defined squares. They were asked to name the letters or point to the target square on each page. The stimuli were graded into four levels of difficulty based on the amount of contrast between the figure and the background. Performance was measured in terms of the maximum level of difficulty at which the participant correctly identified or located the three figures. Contrary to expectations, no significant difference was found between the performance of AD patients and control subjects on texture discrimination tasks vs. luminance discrimination tasks. However, results indicate that AD patients are impaired in performing a task requiring them to locate a texture-defined target of known shape in a noisy background field. By contrast, AD patients show no significant deficit in a task requiring them to locate a texture-defined shape in a known location. This argues that the observed deficit in the location task is not due to a failure in the system that discriminates target texture from background texture (since both location and identification tasks require the same textural discriminations), but rather to an impairment of the system responsible for "finding things" (i.e., locating known targets at unknown locations). This observation suggests that AD patients may suffer selective damage to the dorsal "Where" pathway, which is responsible for localizing objects in space. 相似文献
174.
ERP evidence for the time course of graphic, phonological, and semantic information in Chinese meaning and pronunciation decisions 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
Liu Y Perfetti CA Hart L 《Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition》2003,29(6):1231-1247
Two words that varied in their relationship were presented sequentially to Chinese readers who made meaning and pronunciation decisions. In the meaning task, they decided whether the words had the same meaning. In the pronunciation task, they decided whether the words had the same pronunciation. In both tasks, the word pairs represented 1 of 4 relationships: graphically similar, homophonic, semantically related, or unrelated. Event related potentials (ERP) recordings made from the onset of the 2nd word suggested a temporal unfolding of graphic, phonological, and semantic effects. Specifically, graphically-related pairs produced a smaller P200 in the pronunciation task and a smaller N400 in the meaning task. Homophones produced reduced N400 component with bilateral sources in the meaning task. 相似文献
175.
This longitudinal study examined memory loss in a sample of 391 initially nondemented older adults. Analyses decomposed observed memory loss into decline associated with preclinical dementia, study attrition, terminal decline, and chronological age. Measuring memory as a function of only chronological age failed to provide an adequate representation of cognitive change. Disease progression accounted for virtually all of the memory loss in the 25% of the sample that developed diagnosable dementia. In the remainder of the sample, both chronological age and study attrition contributed to observed memory loss. These results suggest that much of memory loss in aging adults may be attributable to the progression of preclinical dementia and other nonnormative aging processes that are not captured by chronological age. 相似文献
176.
Developments in long-term explicit memory late in the first year of life: behavioral and electrophysiological indices 总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4
Coincident with developments in the temporal-cortical explicit memory network, long-term recall abilities are newly emergent late in the first year of human life. We recorded event-related potentials (ERPs) in 9-month-olds as an index of the integrity of the neural substrate underlying a task thought to reflect explicit memory, namely, deferred imitation. ERP measures of recognition memory 1 week after unique laboratory experiences predicted whether and how much infants recalled of the experiences 1 month later. The findings further imply that memory storage and consolidation processes are a major source of variability in long-term recall memory late in the first year of life. 相似文献
177.
Negy C Shreve TL Jensen BJ Uddin N 《Cultural diversity & ethnic minority psychology》2003,9(4):333-344
Social identity theory (SIT) proposes that the more strongly individuals identify with their group, the less favorable attitudes they hold toward dissimilar groups. In contrast, multicultural theory proposes that affirmation toward one's group--particularly with respect to ethnicity--should correspond with higher levels of acceptance toward dissimilar groups. These competing theories were examined with 486 non-Hispanic White, African American, and Hispanic/Latino university students to determine if support would be found for either theory. Consistent with SIT, levels of ethnic identity correlated significantly with levels of ethnocentrism for Whites and Hispanics but not for African Americans. African Americans obtained significantly higher ethnic identity and self-esteem scores than the other 2 groups. Implications of the findings are discussed. 相似文献
178.
Charles M 《The Psychoanalytic quarterly》2001,70(2):387-416
Psychoanalysis has struggled with issues of touching and being touched, and of holding and being held, since Freud's early essays toward "taking hold" of elusive thoughts through various means. More recently, observations of early dyadic interchanges between caretaker and child have illuminated how facets of the analytic process, such as the quality of gaze, tone, or empathic resonance, affect feelings of "being held" within the object world. These studies interplay with other analytic depictions and the work of affect theorists to show how meanings become represented and manifested over time through verbal versus nonverbal means. The author uses this literature to explore how our capacity to receive and transmit information cross-modally creates an interpenetration of meanings between self and other in the absence of actual physical contact. Clinical illustrations explore some of the meanings and uses of nonphysical modes of touch within the analytic environment. 相似文献
179.
180.
Taking time seriously. A theory of socioemotional selectivity 总被引:25,自引:0,他引:25
Socioemotional selectivity theory claims that the perception of time plays a fundamental role in the selection and pursuit of social goals. According to the theory, social motives fall into 1 of 2 general categories--those related to the acquisition of knowledge and those related to the regulation of emotion. When time is perceived as open-ended, knowledge-related goals are prioritized. In contrast, when time is perceived as limited, emotional goals assume primacy. The inextricable association between time left in life and chronological age ensures age-related differences in social goals. Nonetheless, the authors show that the perception of time is malleable, and social goals change in both younger and older people when time constraints are imposed. The authors argue that time perception is integral to human motivation and suggest potential implications for multiple subdisciplines and research interests in social, developmental, cultural, cognitive, and clinical psychology. 相似文献