首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   6918篇
  免费   272篇
  国内免费   4篇
  2020年   75篇
  2019年   118篇
  2018年   147篇
  2017年   138篇
  2016年   161篇
  2015年   115篇
  2014年   147篇
  2013年   738篇
  2012年   215篇
  2011年   253篇
  2010年   159篇
  2009年   159篇
  2008年   250篇
  2007年   210篇
  2006年   216篇
  2005年   204篇
  2004年   171篇
  2003年   206篇
  2002年   220篇
  2001年   151篇
  2000年   128篇
  1999年   122篇
  1998年   79篇
  1997年   81篇
  1996年   84篇
  1995年   80篇
  1994年   84篇
  1993年   87篇
  1992年   85篇
  1991年   105篇
  1990年   87篇
  1989年   77篇
  1988年   75篇
  1987年   81篇
  1986年   82篇
  1985年   84篇
  1984年   104篇
  1983年   77篇
  1982年   95篇
  1981年   91篇
  1980年   66篇
  1979年   86篇
  1978年   70篇
  1977年   84篇
  1976年   81篇
  1975年   85篇
  1974年   87篇
  1973年   82篇
  1967年   70篇
  1966年   71篇
排序方式: 共有7194条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
951.
Ohne ZusammenfassungIch möchte der Alexander von Humboldt-Stiftung für ihre freundliche Unterstützung danken, mit der sie die Arbeit an diesem Essay gefördert hat.  相似文献   
952.
Chronicle     
  相似文献   
953.
954.
Rural black, economically impoverished Head Start children, whose normal diets were deficient in nutritional requirements, were not consuming the nutritional breakfasts of, to them, unfamiliar foods provided by the county school system. This study investigated a positive simple method to increase the amount of food consumed and thereby ensure proper nutrition. Teachers dispensed sugar-coated cereal and small candies paired with praise contingent on eating behaviors and rewarded children who finished the entire meal with additional treats and praise. Substantial increases were produced in the proportion of meals consumed and in the number of children observed engaged in eating behaviors. These simple traditional behavioral procedures are readily available for any staff working with economically impoverished children as one method of increasing their sampling and consumption of unfamiliar nutritious foods.  相似文献   
955.
956.
957.
A study was conducted to identify codes of nonverbal behavior which could be used by interviewers in a standard field interview to systematically assess deception and emotional states of respondents. Ten male and 10 female subjects were interviewed on topics that had been pretested to arouse in them pleasant involvement, passivity, and unpleasant involvement. Subjects were also instructed to lie to the interviewer on issues of importance to them. Eye contact was measured by observation through a one-way mirror. Smiles, gestures, self-manipulations and postural shifts were scored from videotapes of the interview. The most distinctive patterns of nonverbal cues occurred with unpleasant-involvement and deception questions. The former state was characterized by increases in smiling, self-manipulations, and postural shifts. Deception responses were marked by decreases in smiling and increases in self-manipulations and postural shifts. Results also suggested that eye contact functioned as an intensifier of affect.  相似文献   
958.
Two experiments are reported dealing with choice of fixed or variable time shock schedules with and without signals. Response independent shock was presented on either a fixed-time or a variable-time schedule and subjects could change from one condition to the other for short periods after which the initial condition was automatically reinstated. In the first experiment shocks for both schedules were either unsignalled (6 subjects) or signalled (3 subjects) and all subjects changed from a schedule of variable-time shock to a fixed-time one. Subjects that were given the opportunity to change in the reverse direction did not do so. Opposite findings were obtained in the second experiment (7 subjects) comparing unsignalled fixed-time shock with signalled variable-time shock. Six of the seven subjects changed from the fixed-time schedule to the signalled variable-time one but not the reverse. The data were considered consistent with an analysis of choice based on the reinforcing value of discriminable shock-free periods.  相似文献   
959.
960.
Studies involving personality and situational variables were surveyed. Studies permitting determination of main effects and interactions involving these variables have increased since 1950. In one comparison, situational main effects were significant in 65.5% of the cases, whereas the figure was 31% for individual difference variables and 59.9% for interactions. In another comparison, 35% of situational main effects accounted for more than 10% of the variance, compared with 29% for personality indexes; 19% of the situational variable effects accounted for more than 20% of the variance, compared with 14% of the personality main effects. Low percentages of variance were accounted for by all variables investigated: situational, personality, demographic, and interactions among these variables.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号