首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   24737篇
  免费   1005篇
  国内免费   15篇
  2020年   270篇
  2019年   278篇
  2018年   458篇
  2017年   401篇
  2016年   444篇
  2015年   287篇
  2014年   406篇
  2013年   1882篇
  2012年   707篇
  2011年   777篇
  2010年   419篇
  2009年   502篇
  2008年   699篇
  2007年   676篇
  2006年   636篇
  2005年   571篇
  2004年   537篇
  2003年   548篇
  2002年   511篇
  2001年   844篇
  2000年   834篇
  1999年   635篇
  1998年   268篇
  1997年   255篇
  1993年   241篇
  1992年   519篇
  1991年   471篇
  1990年   522篇
  1989年   435篇
  1988年   421篇
  1987年   407篇
  1986年   421篇
  1985年   478篇
  1984年   399篇
  1983年   326篇
  1982年   269篇
  1981年   244篇
  1979年   365篇
  1978年   294篇
  1977年   257篇
  1976年   246篇
  1975年   347篇
  1974年   394篇
  1973年   398篇
  1972年   302篇
  1971年   264篇
  1969年   247篇
  1968年   314篇
  1967年   277篇
  1966年   307篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
51.
52.
53.
54.
55.
56.
We present a study of the accuracy, consistency, and speed of word naming in a dyslexic boy, JM, who has severe impairments in the ability to use sub-lexical, phonological reading strategies. For words that he can recognise, JM's naming latencies do not differ from those of control subjects matched for reading age, and he is generally consistent from one occasion to the next. He can also match printed homophones with their definitions--a skill that requires access to well-specified orthographic representations. The data are interpreted as evidence for the creation of efficient recognition devices for words within JM's sight vocabulary. However, he shows a continuing inability to use phonological decoding strategies to deal with words that he cannot recognize by sight. Overall we argue our results pose problems for stage models of reading development, and that they may best be interpreted within a connectionist framework of the development of word recognition skills.  相似文献   
57.
Although it is currently popular to model human associative learning using connectionist networks, the mechanism by which their output activations are converted to probabilities of response has received relatively little attention. Several possible models of this decision process are considered here, including a simple ratio rule, a simple difference rule, their exponential versions, and a winner-take-all network. Two categorization experiments that attempt to dissociate these models are reported. Analogues of the experiments were presented to a single-layer, feed-forward, delta-rule network. Only the exponential ratio rule and the winner-take-all architecture, acting on the networks' output activations that corresponded to responses available on test, were capable of fully predicting the mean response results. In addition, unlike the exponential ratio rule, the winner-take-all model has the potential to predict latencies. Further studies will be required to determine whether latencies produced under more stringent conditions conform to the model's predictions.  相似文献   
58.
When forecasting future outcomes, people tend to believe that the outcomes they want to happen are also likely to happen. Despite numerous attempts, few systematic factors have been identified that consistently and robustly reduce wishful thinking (WT) effects. Using elections and sporting event outcomes as contexts, three experiments examined whether taking the perspective of a political rival or opposing fan reduced WT effects. We also examined whether making deliberative (vs. intuitive-based) forecasts was associated with lower WT effects. Online adult samples of U.S. citizens from Mechanical Turk and U.S. college students provided their preferences and forecasts for the U.S. presidential election (Experiments 1 and 2) and a sports competition outcome (Experiment 3). Critically, some participants received perspective taking prompts immediately before providing forecasts. First, results revealed reductions in WT effects when participants engaged in perspective taking. Interestingly, this effect only emerged when intuitive-based forecasts were made first (Experiment 3). Second, intuitive-based forecasts revealed stronger evidence of WT effects. Finally, we found that perspective taking and forming forecasts deliberately promoted a shift in focus away from preferences and toward a consideration of the relative strengths and weaknesses of the entities (i.e., candidates and teams). Theoretical implications for understanding WT effects and applied implications for developing interventions are discussed.  相似文献   
59.
60.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号