全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3914篇 |
免费 | 59篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
3974篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 33篇 |
2019年 | 50篇 |
2018年 | 59篇 |
2017年 | 57篇 |
2016年 | 77篇 |
2015年 | 51篇 |
2014年 | 60篇 |
2013年 | 415篇 |
2012年 | 106篇 |
2011年 | 121篇 |
2010年 | 84篇 |
2009年 | 76篇 |
2008年 | 135篇 |
2007年 | 108篇 |
2006年 | 120篇 |
2005年 | 124篇 |
2004年 | 92篇 |
2003年 | 123篇 |
2002年 | 124篇 |
2001年 | 58篇 |
2000年 | 49篇 |
1999年 | 59篇 |
1998年 | 48篇 |
1997年 | 51篇 |
1996年 | 52篇 |
1995年 | 49篇 |
1994年 | 51篇 |
1993年 | 65篇 |
1992年 | 39篇 |
1991年 | 61篇 |
1990年 | 44篇 |
1989年 | 40篇 |
1988年 | 41篇 |
1987年 | 46篇 |
1986年 | 40篇 |
1985年 | 49篇 |
1984年 | 73篇 |
1983年 | 55篇 |
1982年 | 68篇 |
1981年 | 67篇 |
1980年 | 45篇 |
1979年 | 54篇 |
1978年 | 52篇 |
1977年 | 61篇 |
1976年 | 56篇 |
1975年 | 53篇 |
1974年 | 61篇 |
1973年 | 57篇 |
1967年 | 31篇 |
1966年 | 40篇 |
排序方式: 共有3974条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
101.
102.
103.
Stuart P. Taylor Richard M. Vardaris Allen B. Rawtich Charles B. Gammon Jay W. Cranston Arvin I. Lubetkin 《Aggressive behavior》1976,2(2):153-161
Forty male undergraduates were provoked following their ingestion of high or low doses of either alcohol or delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC). The expression of physical aggression was related to the quantity of alcohol ingested. The high dose of alcohol instigated more intense aggression than the low dose. The high dose of THC, on the other hand, did not increase aggressive behavior. In fact, it tended to produce a weak suppression effect. 相似文献
104.
The segmentation-change model of time perception proposes that individuals engaged in cognitive tasks during a given interval of time retrospectively estimate duration by recalling events that occurred during the interval and inferring each event's duration. Previous research suggests that individuals can recall the number of songs heard during an interval and infer the length of each song, exactly the conditions that foster estimates of duration based on the segmentation-change model. The results of a laboratory experiment indicated that subjects who solved word-search puzzles for 20 min. estimated the duration of the interval to be longer when 8 short songs (<3 min.) as opposed to 4 long songs (6+ min.) were played in the background, regardless of whether the musical format was Contemporary Dance or New Age. Assuming each song represented a distinct segment in memory, these results are consistent with the segmentation-change model. These results suggest that background music may not always reduce estimates of duration by drawing attention away from the passage of time. Instead, background music may actually expand the subjective length of an interval by creating accessible traces in memory, which are retrospectively used to infer duration. 相似文献
105.
Balsam PD Fairhurst S Gallistel CR 《Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes》2006,32(3):284-294
The effects of altering the contingency between the conditioned stimulus (CS) and the unconditioned stimulus (US) on the acquisition of autoshaped responding was investigated by changing the frequency of unsignaled USs during the intertrial interval. The addition of the unsignaled USs had an effect on acquisition speed comparable with that of massing trials. The effects of these manipulations can be understood in terms of their effect on the amount of information (number of bits) that the average CS conveys to the subject about the timing of the next US. The number of reinforced CSs prior to acquisition is inversely related to the information content of the CS. 相似文献
106.
Williams MT Schaefer TL Furay AR Ehrman LA Vorhees CV 《Stress (Amsterdam, Netherlands)》2006,9(3):153-163
We examined the ontogeny of the corticosterone response to (+)-methamphetamine in neonatal rats. In experiment-1, animals were injected with 10 mg/kg of (+)-methamphetamine or saline and plasma corticosterone levels were examined in separate groups 30 or 105 min later on postnatal day (P) 1, 3, 5, 7, 9, 11, 13, 15, 17, or 19. The adrenal response to methamphetamine was best described by a U-shaped function with the nadir of corticosterone release occurring between P7 and P13. Experiment-2 was similar except that the effect of four consecutive days of exposure to (+)-methamphetamine (four times daily at 2 h intervals with 10 mg/kg) was assessed with a single final dose early on the fifth day (i.e. P1-5, 3-7, 5-9, 7-11, 9-13, 11-15, 13-17, 15-19). The 30 min corticosterone response after multiple methamphetamine doses was augmented compared to single exposures, with the exception of the two earliest dosing intervals ending on P5 and P7, where the responses were lower. In addition, at 105 min, the levels of corticosterone were attenuated relative to a single drug administration. With the exception of animals receiving methamphetamine from P15 to P19, thymus weights were unaffected. The data demonstrate that (+)-methamphetamine is a robust activator of corticosterone release in developing animals and this release is extensively modified by age and previous drug exposure. 相似文献
107.
Norman JF Crabtree CE Herrmann M Thompson SR Shular CF Clayton AM 《Perception & psychophysics》2006,68(1):94-101
In two experiments, we investigated the ability of younger and older observers to perceive and discriminate 3-D shape from static and dynamic patterns of binocular disparity. In both experiments, the younger observers' discrimination accuracies were 20% higher than those of the older observers. Despite this quantitative difference, in all other respects the older observers performed similarly to the younger observers. Both age groups were similarly affected by changes in the magnitude of binocular disparity, by reductions in binocular correspondence, and by increases in the speed of stereoscopic motion. In addition, observers in both age groups exhibited an advantage in performance for dynamic stereograms when the patterns of binocular disparity contained significant amounts of correspondence "noise." The process of aging does affect stereopsis, but the effects are quantitative rather than qualitative. 相似文献
108.
A single instrument: Engineering and engineering technology students demonstrating competence in ethics and professional standards 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
This paper details efforts by the Purdue School of Engineering and Technology at Indiana University Purdue University Indianapolis
(IUPUI) to create a single instrument for honors science, technology, engineering and mathematics (STEM) students wishing
to demonstrate competence in the IUPUI Principles of Undergraduate Learning (PUL’s) and Accreditation Board for Engineering
and Technology (ABET) Engineering Accreditation Criterion (EAC) and Technology Accreditation Criterion (TAC) 2, a through
k. Honors courses in Human Behavior, Ethical Decision-Making, Applied Leadership, International Issues and Leadership Theories
and Processes were created along with a specific menu of activities and an assessment rubric based on PUL’s and ABET criteria
to evaluate student performance in the aforementioned courses. Students who complete the series of 18 Honors Credit hours
are eligible for an Honors Certificate in Leadership Studies from the Department of Organizational Leadership and Supervision.
Finally, an accounting of how various university assessment criteria, in this case the IUPUI Principles of Undergraduate Learning,
can be linked to ABET outcomes and prove student competence in both, using the aforementioned courses, menu of items, and
assessment rubrics; these will be analyzed and discussed.
An earlier version of this paper was presented at the 2005 conference, Ethics and Social Responsibility in Engineering and Technology, Linking Workplace Ethics and Education, co-hosted by Gonzaga University and Loyola Marymount University, Los Angeles, California, 9–10 June 2005.
Timothy Diemer is a visiting assistant professor in the Dept. of Organizational Leadership and Supervision and director of
international services in the Purdue School of Engineering and Technology at IUPUI.
Stephen P. Hundley is an Associate Professor of Organizational Leadership and Supervision in the Purdue School of Engineering
and Technology at IUPUI.
Robert M. Wolter is a lecturer in the Dept. of Organizational Leadership and Supervision at IUPUI and teaches ‘Human Behavior
in Organizations’ and ‘Applied Leadership’. 相似文献
109.
Two studies examined the effects of dispositional self-consciousness on reactance. In Experiment 1, men who were high in private self-consciousness displayed greater reactance responses to a coercive communication attempt (as reflected by attitude reversal) than did men lower in private self-consciousness. In contrast, the effect of public self-consciousness in this context was to inhibit the expression of reactance. In Experiment 2, women high in private self-consciousness exhibited greater reactance responses to a self-imposed threat to their freedom of choice (as reflected by equivocation over two choice alternatives) than did women lower in private self-consciousness. The effect of public self-consciousness in this context was negligible. These findings replicate and extend a previous self-awareness and reactance finding; they provide additional evidence that manipulated self-awareness and dispositional self-consciousness converge on the same psychological entity; and they provide additional evidence that there are important differences between the dimensions of private and public self-consciousness. 相似文献
110.
This article focuses on a practical approach to dealing with homosexual concerns within a pastoral context. The manner in which a church worker and a mental health professional can practice co-therapy is detailed with some of the limitations and advantages associated with such a team outlined. 相似文献