全文获取类型
收费全文 | 4529篇 |
免费 | 164篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
4695篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 46篇 |
2019年 | 64篇 |
2018年 | 87篇 |
2017年 | 78篇 |
2016年 | 93篇 |
2015年 | 67篇 |
2014年 | 76篇 |
2013年 | 471篇 |
2012年 | 128篇 |
2011年 | 156篇 |
2010年 | 97篇 |
2009年 | 101篇 |
2008年 | 168篇 |
2007年 | 149篇 |
2006年 | 142篇 |
2005年 | 147篇 |
2004年 | 114篇 |
2003年 | 138篇 |
2002年 | 141篇 |
2001年 | 81篇 |
2000年 | 65篇 |
1999年 | 84篇 |
1998年 | 58篇 |
1997年 | 53篇 |
1996年 | 61篇 |
1995年 | 53篇 |
1994年 | 56篇 |
1993年 | 63篇 |
1992年 | 47篇 |
1991年 | 64篇 |
1990年 | 51篇 |
1989年 | 43篇 |
1988年 | 55篇 |
1987年 | 55篇 |
1986年 | 45篇 |
1985年 | 57篇 |
1984年 | 77篇 |
1983年 | 52篇 |
1982年 | 70篇 |
1981年 | 68篇 |
1980年 | 49篇 |
1979年 | 64篇 |
1978年 | 48篇 |
1977年 | 71篇 |
1976年 | 65篇 |
1975年 | 57篇 |
1974年 | 65篇 |
1973年 | 70篇 |
1972年 | 39篇 |
1966年 | 44篇 |
排序方式: 共有4695条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
91.
92.
93.
Russell M. Church Jonathon D. Crystal Charles E. Collyer 《Behavior research methods》1996,28(2):305-310
Four ways to reduce scientific errors are by tests of equipment and programs, examination of results, peer review, and replication. This article describes various types of errors that may occur and procedures available for the prevention and correction of both unintentional and intentional errors in experiments that use computer programs to generate the stimuli, record the responses, or analyze the data. We describe a case study of a particular experiment that produced a result that has been found to be erroneous. The case study provides additional evidence of the essential importance of replication for the identification and elimination of scientific error. 相似文献
94.
Robert B. Welch Malcolm M. Cohen Charles W. Deroshia 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1996,58(1):22-30
Ten subjects served as their own controls in two conditions of continuous, centrifugally produced hypergravity (+2 Gz) and a 1-G control condition. Before and after exposure, open-loop measures were obtained of (1) motor control, (2) visual localization, and (3) hand-eye coordination. During exposure in the visual feedback/hypergravity condition, subjects received terminal visual error-corrective feedback from their target pointing, and in the no-visual feedback/hypergravity condition they pointed open loop. As expected, the motor control measures for both experimental conditions revealed very short lived underreaching (the muscle-loading effect) at the outset of hypergravity and an equally transient negative aftereffect on returning to 1 G. The substantial (approximately 17°) initial elevator illusion experienced in both hypergravity conditions declined over the course of the exposure period, whether or not visual feedback was provided. This effect was tentatively attributed to habituation of the otoliths. Visual feedback produced a smaller additional decrement and a postexposure negative aftereffect, possible evidence for visual recalibration. Surprisingly, the target-pointing error made during hypergravity in the no-visual-feedback condition was substantially less than that predicted by subjects’ elevator illusion. This finding calls into question the neural outflow model as a complete explanation of this illusion. 相似文献
95.
96.
Timothy H. Monk Jeffrey E. Fookson Jacob Kream Margaret L. Moline Charles P. Pollak Muriel B. Weitzman 《Behavior research methods》1985,17(1):19-26
Cornell University Medical College, Westchester Division, The New York Hospital, White Plains, New York It is well established that there is a complex timekeeping mechanism in the human brain. This mechanism is associated with a variety of physiological and psychological rhythms having a period of about a day, and thus referred to as circadian rhythms. The circadian system has recently been modeled in terms of two underlying oscillators, one much more resistant to changes in routine than the other. These oscillators are considered to be endogenous, that is, internal to the organism, and not reliant for their existence upon changes in the person’s environment or general behavior. They thus continue to run even when the sleep/wake cycle is suspended, as in sustained operations. Thus, by their very nature, sustained operations require the individual to override the inputs that are coming from his or her circadian system (especially the indication that sleep is required). The aim of this paper is to provide a background to the area of circadian rhythms research, including a section on the methodology, so that the impact of the circadian system on sustained operations can be better understood. 相似文献
97.
98.
Daniel L. Roenker Steven K. Wenger Charles P. Thompson Brian Watkins 《Memory & cognition》1978,6(3):288-295
Two factors, level of processing (Craik & Lockhart, 1972) and congruity (Schulman, 1974), known to have large effects on the recall of experimenter-provided responses to questions, were examined in a series of five incidental learning experiments using subject-generated responses. The data show that manipulation of level of processing has the same effect on recall of subjectgenerated responses as it does on experimenter-provided responses. However, the effect of congruity is reversed for subject-generated responses. The data suggest that the difficulty of generating unrelated responses (“incongruous“ items) may account, at least in part, for the failure of the “principle of congruity“ with subject-generated responses. 相似文献
99.
100.
Roy H. Marlowe Charles H. Madsen Charles E. Bowen Robert C. Reardon Patrick E. Logue 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1978,11(1):53-66
This experiment was designed to determine the relative effectiveness of teacher and counselling approaches in the reduction of disruptive or inappropriate classroom behavior. Inappropriate classroom behavior frequencies of 12 academically low achieving, seventh-grade, black male students, with a reported high rate of inappropriate classroom behavior, were recorded. Three groups, with nearly equal mean inappropriate behaviors, were randomly assigned to one of three treatment conditions: behavioral counselling, client-centered counselling, or no counselling. Each counselling group received fifteen 30-minute counselling sessions, at a rate of two to three times a week. In addition to counselling, all students subsequently received teacher approval within the classroom. Results indicated that the teacher was able to reduce inappropriate behavior more than any counselling group. There were also indications that behavioral counselling, but not client-centered counselling, was moderately helpful in reducing inappropriate classroom behavior. 相似文献