全文获取类型
收费全文 | 4529篇 |
免费 | 164篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
4695篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 46篇 |
2019年 | 64篇 |
2018年 | 87篇 |
2017年 | 78篇 |
2016年 | 93篇 |
2015年 | 67篇 |
2014年 | 76篇 |
2013年 | 471篇 |
2012年 | 128篇 |
2011年 | 156篇 |
2010年 | 97篇 |
2009年 | 101篇 |
2008年 | 168篇 |
2007年 | 149篇 |
2006年 | 142篇 |
2005年 | 147篇 |
2004年 | 114篇 |
2003年 | 138篇 |
2002年 | 141篇 |
2001年 | 81篇 |
2000年 | 65篇 |
1999年 | 84篇 |
1998年 | 58篇 |
1997年 | 53篇 |
1996年 | 61篇 |
1995年 | 53篇 |
1994年 | 56篇 |
1993年 | 63篇 |
1992年 | 47篇 |
1991年 | 64篇 |
1990年 | 51篇 |
1989年 | 43篇 |
1988年 | 55篇 |
1987年 | 55篇 |
1986年 | 45篇 |
1985年 | 57篇 |
1984年 | 77篇 |
1983年 | 52篇 |
1982年 | 70篇 |
1981年 | 68篇 |
1980年 | 49篇 |
1979年 | 64篇 |
1978年 | 48篇 |
1977年 | 71篇 |
1976年 | 65篇 |
1975年 | 57篇 |
1974年 | 65篇 |
1973年 | 70篇 |
1972年 | 39篇 |
1966年 | 44篇 |
排序方式: 共有4695条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
101.
Bernardo J Carducci Paul C Cozby Charles D Ward 《Journal of experimental social psychology》1978,14(5):449-457
In an attempt to reconcile conflicting results concerning the effects of sexual arousal on interpersonal evaluations, hypotheses based on adaptation level, arousal-need gratification, and labeling of arousal explanations were tested. The variables of sexual arousal, technique of stimulus material presentation, physical attractiveness of the original stimulus female, and physical attractiveness of the final target person being judged were manipulated in a 2 × 2 × 2 × 2 design. A significant three-way interaction (Arousal × Stimulus Attractiveness × Target Attractiveness) obtained on all measures of interpersonal evaluation was interpreted as supporting a labeling of arousal explanation. It is suggested that subjects first interpret their arousal as being caused by their attraction toward the stimulus person. The characteristics of the stimulus person then become a factor used by subjects when they subsequently make judgments of target females. The adaptation-level and arousal-need gratification hypotheses were not supported. 相似文献
102.
103.
Charles N. Fischer M.A. 《Pastoral Psychology》1978,27(1):62-70
Since Descartes, man has been defined in terms of body and spirit only. Yet there is a third term that is of crucial importance for the founding of a psychology of suffering. The realm of psyche, or soul, is a middle ground between the spirit and the body in which the psychological forces of one's life find expression. A psychological approach to the anguish of the suffering soul can be found within the writings of the early desert fathers as well as in the paintings of Hieronymus Bosch. The penitent must learn patience in the face of his suffering, before he can experience the joy of spiritual maturity.A famous anchorite came to take counsel on high things with the abbot Pastor, but the old man turned away his head, and the anchorite went away aggrieved. His disciple asked Pastor why he had refused to talk with a man so great and of such reputation in his own country, and the old man said that his visitor could speak of heavenly things, but that he himself was of earth. If he had spoken to me of the passions of the soul I could have answered him: but of the things of the spirit I am ignorant.Mr. Fischer is a member of the Psychology Department, University of Dallas, Irving, Texas 75060. 相似文献
104.
105.
106.
107.
The relationship between the Fourier spectra of visual textures (represented by four hypothetical visual channels sensitive to spatial frequencies) and the perceptual appearance of the textures was investigated. Thirty textures were synthesized by combining various spatial frequencies of different amplitudes. Twenty subjects grouped the textures into 2, 3, 4, and 5 groups based on the similarity of their appearance. The groupings were analyzed by means of linear discriminant analysis using the activity of the four channels as predictor variables. The groupings were also examined by multidimensional scaling, and the resulting stimulus configuration was canonically correlated with the channel activity. The results of both analyses indicate a strong relationship between the perceptual appearance of the textures and their Fourier spectra. These findings suport a multiple-channel spatial-frequency model of perception. 相似文献
108.
Beauty Is Only "Name" Deep: The Effect of First-Name On Ratings of Physical Attraction 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
S. Gary Garwood Lewis Cox Valerie Kaplan Neal Wasserman Jefferson L. Sulzer 《Journal of applied social psychology》1980,10(5):431-435
College students ( n = 197) voted to select a beauty queen from among six photographs equivalent in physical attractiveness, as determined by rankings of 35 students from a separate university. Half the photographs bore a desirable first-name and half were assigned an undesirable first-name. As predicted, firstname had a significant effect on physical attraction: Girls with desirable firstnames received 158 votes to 39 for those with undesirable names. These data were discussed in terms of an additive, rather than an interactive, effect, due to the nature of the task. The strength of these findings was related to the study's ecological validity. 相似文献
109.
Statistical significance tests are derived and evaluated for measuring apparent differences between an obtained and an expected binormal ROC curve, between two independent binormal ROC curves, and among groups of independent binormal ROC curves. A binormal ROC curve is described by two parameters which represent the spread of the means and the ratio of the standard deviations of the two underlying Gaussian decision variable distributions. To test the significance of apparent differences between or among ROC curves, approximate χ2 statistics for each of the three tests were constructed from maximum likelihood estimates of the two parameters defining the binormal ROC curve. The performance of each test statistic was evaluated by simulating five-category rating scale data with equal numbers of noise and signal-plus-noise trials (set at 50, 250, and 500) for each of three typical ROC curves. For the significance test involving only one ROC curve, rating scale data were generated from the chance diagonal of the ROC space also. Although test performance was found to be somewhat dependent on the number of trials and on the location of the ROC curve in the ROC space, comparisons of the obtained and expected fractions of (falsely) significant results at various α levels showed the proposed statistical significance tests to be reliable under practical experimental conditions. 相似文献
110.
Lawrence I. Goldberger Lynne M. Ausman R.Charles Boelkins 《Learning and motivation》1980,11(2):185-207
The long-term effects of infantile malnutrition on information processing were studied in squirrel monkeys. From 2 through 8 weeks of age, four low-calorie infants were fed reduced amounts of a high protein diet, limiting their mean body weight gain to 23% of that of four well-fed controls, with all of this gain occurring during the first 2 weeks on the low-calorie diet. Thereafter, both groups received the standard diet ad libitum. At 68 weeks of age, incidental learning was tested by introducing shape cues that were redundant to the solution of a previously learned, two-choice, color discrimination. Removal of the color cues showed that the low-calorie monkeys had learned significantly less about the shape cues than had the controls. The basis for this incidental learning failure was then examined with an embedded-figures dicrimination, to measure attentional ability, and a novel stimulus substitution task, to measure curiosity. The groups did not differ in detecting embedded figures, but the low-calorie monkeys were significantly less likely than controls to select or handle novel stimuli that had been substituted for previously learned discriminative cues. It was concluded that undernutrition in infancy may induce a long-term strategy of only learning information which leads to an immediate reward, while suppressing the acquisition of information due to intrinsic curiosity. 相似文献