首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   19359篇
  免费   737篇
  国内免费   6篇
  2019年   262篇
  2018年   312篇
  2017年   275篇
  2016年   327篇
  2015年   218篇
  2014年   313篇
  2013年   1638篇
  2012年   527篇
  2011年   594篇
  2010年   362篇
  2009年   378篇
  2008年   532篇
  2007年   547篇
  2006年   519篇
  2005年   445篇
  2004年   456篇
  2003年   435篇
  2002年   435篇
  2001年   509篇
  2000年   556篇
  1999年   395篇
  1998年   241篇
  1997年   199篇
  1996年   204篇
  1995年   196篇
  1992年   312篇
  1991年   332篇
  1990年   336篇
  1989年   304篇
  1988年   308篇
  1987年   278篇
  1986年   282篇
  1985年   297篇
  1984年   250篇
  1983年   243篇
  1982年   207篇
  1981年   201篇
  1979年   289篇
  1978年   250篇
  1977年   204篇
  1976年   199篇
  1975年   262篇
  1974年   293篇
  1973年   316篇
  1972年   251篇
  1971年   232篇
  1969年   208篇
  1968年   271篇
  1967年   258篇
  1966年   243篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 17 毫秒
41.
42.
43.
We present a study of the accuracy, consistency, and speed of word naming in a dyslexic boy, JM, who has severe impairments in the ability to use sub-lexical, phonological reading strategies. For words that he can recognise, JM's naming latencies do not differ from those of control subjects matched for reading age, and he is generally consistent from one occasion to the next. He can also match printed homophones with their definitions--a skill that requires access to well-specified orthographic representations. The data are interpreted as evidence for the creation of efficient recognition devices for words within JM's sight vocabulary. However, he shows a continuing inability to use phonological decoding strategies to deal with words that he cannot recognize by sight. Overall we argue our results pose problems for stage models of reading development, and that they may best be interpreted within a connectionist framework of the development of word recognition skills.  相似文献   
44.
45.
46.
47.
Although it is currently popular to model human associative learning using connectionist networks, the mechanism by which their output activations are converted to probabilities of response has received relatively little attention. Several possible models of this decision process are considered here, including a simple ratio rule, a simple difference rule, their exponential versions, and a winner-take-all network. Two categorization experiments that attempt to dissociate these models are reported. Analogues of the experiments were presented to a single-layer, feed-forward, delta-rule network. Only the exponential ratio rule and the winner-take-all architecture, acting on the networks' output activations that corresponded to responses available on test, were capable of fully predicting the mean response results. In addition, unlike the exponential ratio rule, the winner-take-all model has the potential to predict latencies. Further studies will be required to determine whether latencies produced under more stringent conditions conform to the model's predictions.  相似文献   
48.
49.
Native Hawaiian high school students, N = 1779, were surveyed for symptoms of psycho-pathology and suicide attempts in the previous 6 months. Seventy-seven (4.3%) of the students reported making a suicide attempt. There were no significant differences in prevalence rates for males and females. Depression, anxiety, aggression, substance abuse symptoms, and low family support, but not peer support, were significantly correlated with suicide attempts. On logistic regression, depression, substance abuse, and family support independently predicted attempts. The lack of gender difference may indicate a cultural characteristic of the Hawaiian population that differentiates it from mainstream American populations but likens it to the Native American population.  相似文献   
50.
This research examines how maximizers make decisions when they must trade-off between desirability and feasibility attributes. Across four studies, we demonstrate that maximizers tend to prefer choices offering more desirability to those offering more feasibility and respond more favorably to a product's advertising when it highlights desirability more than feasibility attributes. Furthermore, we show that maximizers' focus on outcomes rather than processes drives their preference for desirability, such that changing from an outcome to a process focus can redirect their interest from desirability to feasibility. By contrast, satisficers do not prefer products higher in desirability to those higher in feasibility and are not more receptive to ads highlighting desirability attributes. Furthermore, because satisficers may focus on both the outcome and the process, priming either one is redundant and does not alter their preference for desirability or feasibility.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号