首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   23257篇
  免费   1004篇
  国内免费   13篇
  2020年   299篇
  2019年   299篇
  2018年   477篇
  2017年   441篇
  2016年   472篇
  2015年   311篇
  2014年   427篇
  2013年   1793篇
  2012年   748篇
  2011年   760篇
  2010年   405篇
  2009年   499篇
  2008年   665篇
  2007年   646篇
  2006年   594篇
  2005年   515篇
  2004年   493篇
  2003年   485篇
  2002年   439篇
  2001年   814篇
  2000年   806篇
  1999年   598篇
  1998年   248篇
  1997年   220篇
  1996年   203篇
  1992年   497篇
  1991年   434篇
  1990年   491篇
  1989年   411篇
  1988年   389篇
  1987年   372篇
  1986年   396篇
  1985年   443篇
  1984年   348篇
  1983年   292篇
  1982年   229篇
  1979年   339篇
  1978年   266篇
  1977年   210篇
  1976年   204篇
  1975年   302篇
  1974年   357篇
  1973年   357篇
  1972年   281篇
  1971年   250篇
  1970年   211篇
  1969年   233篇
  1968年   291篇
  1967年   255篇
  1966年   274篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
51.
52.
Abstract

As more organizations implement multinational strategies, sales managers leading sales forces encounter complex cultural challenges that affect relationships, processes, and outcomes. We undertake a qualitative study with the objective of understanding the sales manager–salesperson relationship when the sales manager is leading sales representatives located in other cultures. Because of the significant size and growth of Asian countries, we focus our study on the Asia-Pacific Rim region. In-depth interviews conducted with 21 sales managers working for a large multinational technology firm in our focal region provide the data for our analysis. Using a grounded theory approach, we identify five key themes: building and sustaining cross-cultural relationships, cross-cultural communication effectiveness, acquisition and maintenance of trust across cultures, language, and decision-making. From our findings, research propositions are offered and implications for researchers and practitioners are discussed.  相似文献   
53.
54.
55.
56.
57.
58.
59.
Although it is currently popular to model human associative learning using connectionist networks, the mechanism by which their output activations are converted to probabilities of response has received relatively little attention. Several possible models of this decision process are considered here, including a simple ratio rule, a simple difference rule, their exponential versions, and a winner-take-all network. Two categorization experiments that attempt to dissociate these models are reported. Analogues of the experiments were presented to a single-layer, feed-forward, delta-rule network. Only the exponential ratio rule and the winner-take-all architecture, acting on the networks' output activations that corresponded to responses available on test, were capable of fully predicting the mean response results. In addition, unlike the exponential ratio rule, the winner-take-all model has the potential to predict latencies. Further studies will be required to determine whether latencies produced under more stringent conditions conform to the model's predictions.  相似文献   
60.
When forecasting future outcomes, people tend to believe that the outcomes they want to happen are also likely to happen. Despite numerous attempts, few systematic factors have been identified that consistently and robustly reduce wishful thinking (WT) effects. Using elections and sporting event outcomes as contexts, three experiments examined whether taking the perspective of a political rival or opposing fan reduced WT effects. We also examined whether making deliberative (vs. intuitive-based) forecasts was associated with lower WT effects. Online adult samples of U.S. citizens from Mechanical Turk and U.S. college students provided their preferences and forecasts for the U.S. presidential election (Experiments 1 and 2) and a sports competition outcome (Experiment 3). Critically, some participants received perspective taking prompts immediately before providing forecasts. First, results revealed reductions in WT effects when participants engaged in perspective taking. Interestingly, this effect only emerged when intuitive-based forecasts were made first (Experiment 3). Second, intuitive-based forecasts revealed stronger evidence of WT effects. Finally, we found that perspective taking and forming forecasts deliberately promoted a shift in focus away from preferences and toward a consideration of the relative strengths and weaknesses of the entities (i.e., candidates and teams). Theoretical implications for understanding WT effects and applied implications for developing interventions are discussed.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号