全文获取类型
收费全文 | 271篇 |
免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
273篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 1篇 |
2023年 | 1篇 |
2022年 | 4篇 |
2020年 | 7篇 |
2019年 | 8篇 |
2018年 | 9篇 |
2017年 | 14篇 |
2016年 | 11篇 |
2015年 | 7篇 |
2014年 | 10篇 |
2013年 | 34篇 |
2012年 | 22篇 |
2011年 | 17篇 |
2010年 | 11篇 |
2009年 | 8篇 |
2008年 | 21篇 |
2007年 | 16篇 |
2006年 | 14篇 |
2005年 | 17篇 |
2004年 | 6篇 |
2003年 | 7篇 |
2002年 | 4篇 |
2001年 | 4篇 |
2000年 | 4篇 |
1999年 | 5篇 |
1998年 | 1篇 |
1997年 | 2篇 |
1996年 | 1篇 |
1995年 | 1篇 |
1994年 | 1篇 |
1992年 | 1篇 |
1982年 | 1篇 |
1978年 | 1篇 |
1971年 | 1篇 |
1954年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有273条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
91.
Chad M. Bauman 《Pastoral Psychology》2018,67(2):195-198
This article provides an introduction to the essays in this issue on the Oxford Handbook of Religious Conversion, edited by Lewis Rambo and Charles Farhadian. After briefly discussing the origins of these essays in a session sponsored by the Religious Conversions Group at the 2015 American Academy of Religion annual meeting, as well as some of the broad historical trends and problems in the study of conversion(s), the essay provides a summary of each author’s contributions and suggests that one particular topic—healing—deserves more attention in the study of conversion than it has heretofore enjoyed. 相似文献
92.
93.
White AE Kenrick DT Li YJ Mortensen CR Neuberg SL Cohen AB 《Journal of personality and social psychology》2012,103(4):622-634
Humans have perennially faced threats of violence from other humans and have developed functional strategies for surviving those threats. Five studies examined the relation between threats of violence and agreeableness at the level of nations, individuals, and situations. People living in countries with higher military spending (Study 1) and those who chronically perceive threats from others (Study 2) were more agreeable. However, this threat-linked agreeableness was selective (Studies 3-5). Participants primed with threat were more agreeable and willing to help familiar others but were less agreeable and willing to help unfamiliar others. Additionally, people from large families, for whom affiliation may be a salient response to threat, were more likely than people from small families to shift in agreeableness. Returning to the national level, military spending was associated with increased trust in ingroup members but decreased trust in outgroups. Together, these findings demonstrate that agreeableness is selectively modulated by threats of violence. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved). 相似文献
94.
Chad L. Hagans Greg J. Neimeyer C. Robert Goodholm Jr. 《Journal of constructivist psychology》2013,26(2):155-173
The effect of personal construct elicitation methods on construct content and structure was addressed in two studies. In Study 1, a between-subjects design was used to compare Kelly's (1955) original Triadic Difference method of elicitation with a Dyadic Opposite method. Study 2 used a fully-crossed, mixed factorial design to compare Kelly's (1955) Triadic Difference method with Epting, Suchman, and Nickeson's (1971) Triadic Opposite method. Results showed that "difference" methods of construct elicitation produced significantly higher levels of construct differentiation, lower numbers of positive emergent construct poles, and less socially undesirable implicit construct poles than "opposite" methods (see also Caputi & Reddy, in press). Implications of these findings are discussed in relation to a critical reappraisal of repertory grid methods. 相似文献
95.
Jerry I. Shaw Jon E. Bergen Chad A. Brown Maureen E. Gallagher 《The Journal of general psychology》2013,140(2):157-164
Three explanations were explored for the finding that people prefer the middle option rather than the extremes when choosing from an array of similar options. In Study 1, 68% chose the middle item from a set of three highlighters and three surveys, whereas 32% chose an item from either end, p < .0001. In Study 2, 71% selected the middle chair from a row of three chairs that were either all empty, or had a backpack occupying either one of the two end chairs, p < .0001. These results support a minimal mental effort principle rather than a preference for symmetry rule. In Study 3, 54.2% recalled more graphic items from the center poster of a 3-poster collage, whereas 31.3% and 14.5% recalled more items from the left and right posters, respectively, p < .004. These findings lend additional support to a focus of attention explanation. 相似文献
96.
Brenton W. McMenamin Jasmine Radue Joanna Trask Kristin Huskamp Daniel Kersten Chad J. Marsolek 《Motivation and emotion》2013,37(3):609-622
Object classification can be facilitated if simple diagnostic features can be used to determine class membership. Previous studies have found that simple shapes may be diagnostic for emotional content and automatically alter the allocation of visual attention. In the present study, we analyzed whether color is diagnostic of emotional content and tested whether emotionally diagnostic hues alter the allocation of visual attention. Reddish-yellow hues are more common in (i.e., diagnostic of) emotional images, particularly images with positive emotional content. An exogenous cueing paradigm was employed to test whether these diagnostic hues orient attention differently from other hues due to the emotional diagnosticity. In two experiments, we found that participants allocated attention differently to diagnostic hues than to non-diagnostic hues, in a pattern indicating a broadening of spatial attention when cued with diagnostic hues. Moreover, the attentional broadening effect was predicted by self-reported measures of affective style, linking the behavioral effect to emotional processes. These results confirm the existence and use of diagnostic features for the rapid detection of emotional content. 相似文献
97.
Reviewed by Chad S. Dodson Vikram K. Jaswal 《Journal of cognition and development》2013,14(3):390-392
Patricia Bauer's impressive book, Remembering the Times of Our Lives: Memory in Infancy and Beyond, traces the development of autobiographical memory from infancy to adulthood. Drawing on a variety of different lines of evidence, ranging from brain development to cognitive development, this book reviews the fundamental questions (e.g., why do we remember so little about early childhood?) and proposes some provocative answers. Both experts and novices will have much to gain from this volume. 相似文献
98.
Chad Carmichael 《Philosophical Studies》2010,150(3):373-389
In this paper, I argue that there are universals. I begin (Sect. 1) by proposing a sufficient condition for a thing’s being a universal. I then argue (Sect. 2) that some truths exist necessarily. Finally, I argue (Sects. 3 and 4) that these truths are structured entities having constituents that meet the proposed sufficient condition for being universals. 相似文献
99.
Larry L. Jacoby Christopher N. Wahlheim Matthew G. Rhodes Karen A. Daniels Chad S. Rogers 《Memory & cognition》2010,38(6):820-829
Results from two experiments revealed that prior experience with proactive interference (PI) diminished PI’s effects for both
young and older adults. Participants were given two rounds of experience, with different materials, in a situation that produced
PI. Comparisons with a control condition showed that the effects of PI on accuracy and on high-confidence intrusion errors
(false memory) were reduced on the second round, as compared with those on the first. Also, the ability of confidence to diagnose
accuracy of responding improved across rounds. Effects of prior experience with PI depended on feedback given at the time
of test (Experiment 1). At least in part, the diminishment of PI resulted from participants’ allocating more attention to
interference items during study in the second round than in the first (Experiment 2). Implications of the results for interpreting
age differences in PI and false memory are discussed. 相似文献
100.
Jerry Suls John Chambers Zlatan Krizan Chad R. Mortensen Bryan Koestner Kathryn Bruchmann 《Organizational behavior and human decision processes》2010
In six experiments, we tested four explanations for the better/worse-than-average effect (B/WTA) by manipulating the number of items comprising the target or referent of direct comparison. A single-item target tended to be rated more extremely than a single-item or a multi-item referent (Experiments 1–3). No B/WTA was obtained, however, when a multi-item target was compared with either a single- or multi-item referent (Experiments 4 and 5). A bias favoring a multi-item target was found only if cohesiveness among the items was increased through instructions (Experiment 6). The Unique-Attributes Hypothesis generally provided the best explanation the findings; the focalism explanation also demonstrated some empirical viability. The results suggest that important preferential decision-making outcomes can be affected by both the number of items and whether items are strategically manipulated to serve as targets or referents of comparison. 相似文献