首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   8130篇
  免费   410篇
  国内免费   5篇
  2024年   17篇
  2023年   100篇
  2022年   136篇
  2021年   181篇
  2020年   218篇
  2019年   243篇
  2018年   390篇
  2017年   384篇
  2016年   334篇
  2015年   221篇
  2014年   259篇
  2013年   1070篇
  2012年   583篇
  2011年   592篇
  2010年   382篇
  2009年   239篇
  2008年   479篇
  2007年   398篇
  2006年   361篇
  2005年   300篇
  2004年   209篇
  2003年   197篇
  2002年   180篇
  2001年   81篇
  2000年   120篇
  1999年   79篇
  1998年   46篇
  1997年   33篇
  1996年   36篇
  1995年   32篇
  1994年   30篇
  1993年   27篇
  1992年   45篇
  1991年   40篇
  1990年   33篇
  1989年   38篇
  1988年   32篇
  1987年   26篇
  1986年   18篇
  1985年   22篇
  1984年   21篇
  1983年   18篇
  1982年   17篇
  1979年   19篇
  1976年   17篇
  1974年   16篇
  1973年   17篇
  1972年   16篇
  1971年   19篇
  1968年   23篇
排序方式: 共有8545条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
991.
This study examined a moderated mediation model testing whether dysmorphic concern is related to behaviour altered to change appearance. This relationship is potentially mediated by depressive symptomatology (dysphoria and self-esteem) and ideas of reference about “laughing, commenting” and “attention, appearance,” and each mediated relationship moderated by sex. The sample was made up of 3377 adolescents from 12 to 18 years old (Mage = 14.52; SD = 1.65, 56.5% girls). The results showed that dysphoria and ideas of reference about “laughing, commenting” and “attention, appearance” partially mediated the relationship between dysmorphic concern and behavioural impairment related to body image. The relationship with dysphoria was moderated by sex, such that the mediation effect was stronger in girls than in boys. This result implies that girls who are worried about some characteristic of their appearance and show dysphoria are at greater risk of altered behaviour involving avoidance or controlling their appearance than boys. In addition, a possible risk of body dysmorphic disorder (3.45% of the sample) was found, with very prominent hiding behaviour using clothing or control behaviours, such as frequent weighing and looking at oneself in the mirror too much. Implications of these findings are discussed.  相似文献   
992.
We examined the relationship between executive functions and both factual and conceptual learning of science, specifically chemistry, in early adolescence. Sixty‐three pupils in their second year of secondary school (aged 12–13 years) participated. Pupils completed tasks of working memory (Spatial Working Memory), inhibition (Stop‐Signal), attention set‐shifting (ID/ED), and planning (Stockings of Cambridge), from the CANTAB. They also participated in a chemistry teaching session, practical, and assessment on the topic of acids and alkalis designed specifically for this study. Executive function data were related to (1) the chemistry assessment which included aspects of factual and conceptual learning and (2) a recent school science exam. Correlational analyses between executive functions and both the chemistry assessment and science grades revealed that science achievements were significantly correlated with working memory. Linear regression analysis revealed that visuospatial working memory ability was predictive of chemistry performance. Interestingly, this relationship was observed solely in relation to the conceptual learning condition of the assessment highlighting the role of executive functions in understanding and applying knowledge about what is learned within science teaching.  相似文献   
993.
Preschoolers are well known for their poor working memory (WM) performance. This could result from goal neglect, which would hamper the setting of maintenance strategies. Previous studies have shown that preschoolers’ WM performance can be improved in game-like tasks, because they provide cues to support goal maintenance. However, in these studies, it was unclear what features of the task (either the main toy or the motor activity required by the game) provide efficient cues. The aim of the present study was to disentangle the two features to examine cue effects in 5- to 7-year-old children. No improvement of WM performance was observed when the toy was a potential goal cue, whereas the motor activity had a detrimental effect in all age groups. The latter effect could result from a distraction of attention from attention-based maintenance activities. Hence, preschoolers' poor WM performance would not be fundamentally due to goal neglect.  相似文献   
994.
995.
996.
Book Review     
  相似文献   
997.
998.
999.
1000.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号