首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   218篇
  免费   12篇
  230篇
  2023年   1篇
  2022年   2篇
  2021年   7篇
  2020年   10篇
  2019年   7篇
  2018年   7篇
  2017年   10篇
  2016年   5篇
  2015年   9篇
  2014年   17篇
  2013年   22篇
  2012年   14篇
  2011年   6篇
  2010年   8篇
  2009年   7篇
  2008年   9篇
  2007年   13篇
  2006年   11篇
  2005年   10篇
  2004年   7篇
  2003年   5篇
  2002年   9篇
  2001年   3篇
  2000年   3篇
  1999年   2篇
  1998年   3篇
  1997年   2篇
  1996年   1篇
  1995年   4篇
  1994年   1篇
  1993年   4篇
  1992年   2篇
  1990年   4篇
  1989年   1篇
  1987年   1篇
  1978年   1篇
  1977年   1篇
  1975年   1篇
排序方式: 共有230条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
111.
ABSTRACT

Animals – both tame and wild, as metaphors and as real presences – populate many of More’s works. In this essay, I show that, from the early Psychodia Platonica to the Divine Dialogues, animals are at the core of key metaphysical issues that reverberate on the levels of psychology and ethics. In particular I discuss three main aspects: (1) the role of animals in More’s critique of atheism, both as safeguard for the body–soul interaction and as proofs of divine providence in nature; (2) the problem of evil in the universe, and how to justify the existence of ‘evil’ animals in particular; (3) the differentiation between animals and humans, especially on the basis of their respective possibility of attaining happiness. In all three cases, I argue that More attempts to ‘tame’ the nature of animals, and yet that he is aware that ‘animality’ remains partly untamed.  相似文献   
112.
"Automatic imitation" is a type of stimulus-response compatibility effect in which the topographical features of task-irrelevant action stimuli facilitate similar, and interfere with dissimilar, responses. This article reviews behavioral, neurophysiological, and neuroimaging research on automatic imitation, asking in what sense it is "automatic" and whether it is "imitation." This body of research reveals that automatic imitation is a covert form of imitation, distinct from spatial compatibility. It also indicates that, although automatic imitation is subject to input modulation by attentional processes, and output modulation by inhibitory processes, it is mediated by learned, long-term sensorimotor associations that cannot be altered directly by intentional processes. Automatic imitation provides an important tool for the investigation of the mirror neuron system, motor mimicry, and complex forms of imitation. It is a new behavioral phenomenon, comparable with the Stroop and Simon effects, providing strong evidence that even healthy adult humans are prone, in an unwilled and unreasoned way, to copy the actions of others.  相似文献   
113.
According to the active system consolidation theory, memory consolidation during sleep relies on the reactivation of newly encoded memory representations. This reactivation is orchestrated by the interplay of sleep slow oscillations, spindles, and theta, which are in turn modulated by certain neurotransmitters like GABA to enable long-lasting plastic changes in the memory store. Here we asked whether the GABAergic system and associated changes in sleep oscillations are functionally related to memory reactivation during sleep. We administered the GABAA agonist zolpidem (10 mg) in a double-blind placebo-controlled study. To specifically focus on the effects on memory reactivation during sleep, we experimentally induced such reactivations by targeted memory reactivation (TMR) with learning-associated reminder cues presented during post-learning slow-wave sleep (SWS). Zolpidem significantly enhanced memory performance with TMR during sleep compared with placebo. Zolpidem also increased the coupling of fast spindles and theta to slow oscillations, although overall the power of slow spindles and theta was reduced compared with placebo. In an uncorrected exploratory analysis, memory performance was associated with slow spindle responses to TMR in the zolpidem condition, whereas it was associated with fast spindle responses in placebo. These findings provide tentative first evidence that GABAergic activity may be functionally implicated in memory reactivation processes during sleep, possibly via its effects on slow oscillations, spindles and theta as well as their interplay.

Sleep supports the consolidation of newly acquired memories (Mednick et al. 2011; Klinzing et al. 2019). According to the active system consolidation theory, new memories and their associated neuronal activation patterns become spontaneously reactivated (replayed) following learning in the sleeping brain (Wilson and McNaughton 1994; Diekelmann and Born 2010). These reactivations allow for the redistribution and integration of the memory representations from hippocampal to neocortical sites for long-term storage (Rasch and Born 2007; Klinzing et al. 2019). Memory reactivation during sleep has been proposed to rely on the synchronized interplay of electrophysiological oscillations characteristic of non–rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep, mainly neocortical slow oscillations (SOs, <1 Hz), thalamocortical spindles (9–15 Hz), and hippocampal ripples (80–200 Hz) (Mölle et al. 2009; Staresina et al. 2015; Helfrich et al. 2018; Ngo et al. 2020). Particularly, sleep spindles and their intricate phase coupling to SO have been suggested to be mechanistically involved in memory consolidation processes during sleep (Ulrich 2016; Antony et al. 2019). It has been proposed that memories become reinstated by spindle events, specifically during the up-state of slow oscillations, allowing for the flow of information between different brain sites as well as the induction of lasting structural and functional plastic changes in the learning-associated neuronal networks (Rosanova and Ulrich 2005; Peyrache and Seibt 2020). In addition to sleep spindles, neocortical and hippocampal theta activity (4–8 Hz) is also phase-locked to SO during NREM sleep (Gonzalez et al. 2018; Cox et al. 2019; Krugliakova et al. 2020), and this coupling has been related to memory consolidation during sleep (Schreiner et al. 2018).A number of neuromodulators seem to be involved in the generation of sleep spindles, SO and associated memory processing, most notably GABA (γ-aminobutyric acid), which is the major inhibitory neurotransmitter (Lancel 1999; Ulrich et al. 2018). Sleep spindles and sleep-dependent memory processing can be boosted by targeting the GABAergic system pharmacologically (Mednick et al. 2013). Zolpidem is one of the most frequently used drugs in this regard, binding to GABAA receptors at the same location as benzodiazepines, thereby acting as a GABAA receptor agonist (Lemmer 2007). Zolpidem increases the time spent in slow-wave sleep (SWS) and reduces the amount of rapid eye movement (REM) sleep (Kanno et al. 2000; Uchimura et al. 2006; Zhang et al. 2020). Zolpidem also increases the density and power of sleep spindles (Dijk et al. 2010; Lundahl et al. 2012; Mednick et al. 2013; Niknazar et al. 2015; Zhang et al. 2020) as well as the coupling of spindles to SO (Niknazar et al. 2015; Zhang et al. 2020), and it was further found to enhance declarative memory consolidation during sleep, with postsleep performance improvements being associated with higher spindle density and spindle power as well as with SO–spindle coupling (Kaestner et al. 2013; Mednick et al. 2013; Zhang et al. 2020).However, it remains unclear whether the changes in sleep stages, sleep spindles, and SO–spindle coupling after pharmacological manipulation with zolpidem are functionally related to the mechanisms underlying sleep-dependent memory consolidation such as memory reactivation. Over the last few years, targeted memory reactivation (TMR) has been increasingly applied to manipulate memory reactivation during sleep experimentally by presenting learning-associated reminder cues like odors or sounds (Oudiette and Paller 2013; Hu et al. 2020; Klinzing and Diekelmann 2020). TMR biases sleep-related neuronal replay events toward the reactivated memory contents (Lewis and Bendor 2019) and enhances subsequent recall performance (Rudoy et al. 2009; Diekelmann et al. 2011; Schreiner et al. 2015; Cairney et al. 2018). Although a few studies observed modulations of SOs (Rihm et al. 2014), sleep spindles (Cox et al. 2014), and SO–spindle coupling (Bar et al. 2020) with TMR during sleep, studies on the role of specific neurotransmitters and particularly on the role of GABAergic neurotransmission and associated changes in sleep oscillations for targeted memory reactivation are entirely lacking. One previous study tested the effect of pharmacologically increased GABAergic activity by administering the benzodiazepine clonazepam after cued reactivation of a declarative memory during wakefulness (Rodríguez et al. 2013). Clonazepam increased memory performance when it was administered after reactivation with an incomplete reminder cue, suggesting that increasing GABAergic neurotransmission may enhance the restabilization of reactivated declarative memories in humans during wakefulness.In the present study, we tested the effect of modulating GABAergic activity with zolpidem on targeted memory reactivation during sleep and associated changes in sleep spindles as well as SO–spindle and SO–theta coupling. We hypothesized that zolpidem enhances the beneficial effects of targeted memory reactivation on memory performance and that this enhancement is associated with increases in spindle density, spindle power, SO–spindle coupling, and possibly SO–theta coupling, and the amount of SWS. Participants were trained on a memory task including 30 sound–word associations in the evening (Forcato et al. 2020) and received an oral dose of 10 mg zolpidem (n = 11) or placebo (n = 11) after training before a full night of sleep in the sleep lab (Fig. 1). During the night, incomplete reminder cues (sounds + first syllable of the associated words) were played again via in-ear headphones during SWS. The next morning, participants were trained on an interference memory task to probe the stability of the original memory, which was tested 30 min later.Open in a separate windowFigure 1.Experimental design and memory task. (A) All subjects took part in a training session at ∼22.30, were administered with placebo (n = 11) or 10 mg of zolpidem (n = 11) before going to bed at 23:00, and received targeted memory reactivation during the first SWS period. After ∼8 h of sleep, in the morning, subjects learned an interference task and were tested on the original memory task in a testing session 30 min after the interference task. (B) Training: First, subjects were presented with 30 sound–word associations for learning. For each association, the sound was presented first for 2900 msec. The sound then continued accompanied by the word written on the screen and spoken aloud for 1500 msec. After a 4000-msec break, the next association was presented in the same way. After all associations were presented once, participants completed an immediate cued recall test. For each association, the sound was presented for 2900 msec. The sound then continued accompanied by the first syllable of the associated word for 1500 msec. Participants were then given 5000 msec to say the complete word aloud (sound continued during the entire period). Independently of their response, the correct answer was then presented on the screen and via headphones for 1500 msec. Reactivation: Each sound was first presented alone for an average of 2900 msec; the sound then continued accompanied by the first syllable of each word for another 1500 msec. After a 7000-msec break, the next sound–syllable pair was presented until all 30 pairs had been presented once. Testing: Each sound was presented for 500 msec and then the sound continued and subjects had 5000 msec to say the associated word aloud. After a break of 4000 msec, the procedure continued for the rest of the 30 associations. Adapted from Forcato et al. (2020).  相似文献   
114.
The Satir Model and Cultural Sensitivity: A Hong Kong Reflection   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
This paper addresses cultural sensitivity in applying the Satir model in the Hong Kong Chinese cultural setting, focusing on congruent communication. The authors contend that Satir was sensitive to the demands of culture. Her focus on individuality, equality, and freedom responds aptly to the egalitarian individualist ethos of the United States. In Hong Kong the family is generally dominated by hierarchical collectivist values. Applying the Satir model as it is being practiced in the United States totally ignores prevailing cultural conditions. Following the spirit of Satir, the authors propose the adoption of an emic approach, drawing inspiration and resource from traditional Chinese culture and maintaining continuity with hierarchical collectivism while protecting individual quests for equality, freedom, and independence.  相似文献   
115.
The active intermodal mapping hypothesis suggests that intentional imitation is mediated by a highly efficient, special-purpose mechanism of actor-centered movement encoding. In the present study, using methods from stimulus-response (S-R) compatibility research, we found no evidence to support this hypothesis. In two experiments, the performance of adult participants instructed to imitate actorcentered spatial properties of head, arm, and leg movements was affected by task-irrelevant, egocentric spatial cues. In Experiment 1, participants imitated using the same side of their bodies as did the model, and performance was less accurate when egocentric stimulus location was response incompatible than when it was response compatible. This effect was reversed in Experiment 2 when participants imitated using the opposite side of their bodies. These findings, in line with general process theories of imitation, imply that intentional imitation is mediated by the same processes that mediate responding to inanimate stimuli on the basis of arbitrary S-R mappings.  相似文献   
116.
117.
This is an analysis of practical theology in a parish, responding to organized racism in the local area. The involvement in civil action, as well as the theological aspects of such work are considered and discussed in relation to Scandinavian creation theology. The need for radical practice is discussed in relation to vocation and the love of the neighbor, even when such practice becomes uncomfortable and even threatening.  相似文献   
118.
This study aimed to examine the effects and feasibility of a virtual screen-based stress management programme (V-DESSERTS) on inpatients with mental disorders. A single-blinded, pilot randomised controlled trial was conducted in a tertiary hospital in Singapore. Convenience sampling was used and participants were randomised into either the intervention group or the waitlisted control group (WL). The intervention group received individual-based, twice-daily sessions of the programme. Each session comprised education and virtual screen-based relaxation practice. Data were collected through self-reported questionnaires and physiological measures. The intervention group showed a significant increase in perceived relaxation and knowledge in comparison with the WL group. However, inconclusive results were observed on subjective and objective stress. The findings in this study indicated that the V-DESSERTS programme is feasible to be implemented for patients with schizophrenia, depression and bipolar disorders.  相似文献   
119.
This multinational study simultaneously tested three prominent hypotheses—universal disposition, cultural relativity, and livability—that explained differences in subjective well‐being across nations. We performed multilevel structural equation modeling to examine the hypothesized relationships at both individual and cultural levels in 33 nations. Participants were 6,753 university students (2,215 men; 4,403 women; 135 did not specify), and the average age of the entire sample was 20.97 years (SD = 2.39). Both individual‐ and cultural‐level analyses supported the universal disposition and cultural relativity hypotheses by revealing significant associations of subjective well‐being with Extraversion, Neuroticism, and independent self‐construal. In addition, interdependent self‐construal was positively related to life satisfaction at the individual level only, whereas aggregated negative affect was positively linked with aggregate levels of Extraversion and interdependent self‐construal at the cultural level only. Consistent with the livability hypothesis, gross national income (GNI) was related to aggregate levels of negative affect and life satisfaction. There was also a quadratic relationship between GNI and aggregated positive affect. Our findings reveal that universal disposition, cultural self‐construal, and national income can elucidate differences in subjective well‐being, but the multilevel analyses advance the literature by yielding new findings that cannot be identified in studies using individual‐level analyses alone.  相似文献   
120.
This paper longitudinally studies the emergence and evolution of the first symbolic uses of objects in Mexican children. We observed eight children in triadic interaction with one of their parents and 10 objects in a semi-structured situation at nine, 12, 15 and 18 months old. The children began to use objects symbolically at 12 months, and the duration and frequency increased with age. The highest percentage of the total frequency of symbolic uses, of the four levels identified, was level 1. The frequency of level 4, where two or more symbolic actions occur one after the other, giving rise to ‘symbolic narratives’, increased according to age. These data confirm that knowledge of the rules of canonical uses of objects are the meanings which first symbols base themselves on and that children use the symbolic uses that they are aware of to create ‘symbolic narratives’.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号