全文获取类型
收费全文 | 793篇 |
免费 | 15篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
810篇 |
出版年
2018年 | 9篇 |
2017年 | 14篇 |
2016年 | 9篇 |
2015年 | 13篇 |
2014年 | 11篇 |
2013年 | 94篇 |
2012年 | 23篇 |
2011年 | 13篇 |
2010年 | 13篇 |
2009年 | 14篇 |
2008年 | 29篇 |
2007年 | 23篇 |
2006年 | 27篇 |
2005年 | 28篇 |
2004年 | 21篇 |
2003年 | 27篇 |
2002年 | 23篇 |
2001年 | 13篇 |
2000年 | 12篇 |
1999年 | 17篇 |
1998年 | 14篇 |
1997年 | 9篇 |
1995年 | 10篇 |
1994年 | 8篇 |
1993年 | 8篇 |
1992年 | 8篇 |
1991年 | 11篇 |
1990年 | 8篇 |
1989年 | 9篇 |
1988年 | 13篇 |
1987年 | 8篇 |
1985年 | 10篇 |
1984年 | 14篇 |
1983年 | 12篇 |
1982年 | 14篇 |
1981年 | 9篇 |
1980年 | 10篇 |
1979年 | 10篇 |
1978年 | 11篇 |
1977年 | 13篇 |
1976年 | 13篇 |
1975年 | 9篇 |
1974年 | 14篇 |
1973年 | 13篇 |
1972年 | 7篇 |
1971年 | 10篇 |
1970年 | 7篇 |
1968年 | 12篇 |
1967年 | 8篇 |
1966年 | 11篇 |
排序方式: 共有810条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
61.
Ruth E. Propper Lauren J. O’Donnell Stephen Whalen Yanmei Tie Isaiah H. Norton Ralph O. Suarez Lilla Zollei Alireza Radmanesh Alexandra J. Golby 《Brain and cognition》2010
The present study examined the relationship between hand preference degree and direction, functional language lateralization in Broca’s and Wernicke’s areas, and structural measures of the arcuate fasciculus. Results revealed an effect of degree of hand preference on arcuate fasciculus structure, such that consistently-handed individuals, regardless of the direction of hand preference, demonstrated the most asymmetric arcuate fasciculus, with larger left versus right arcuate, as measured by DTI. Functional language lateralization in Wernicke’s area, measured via fMRI, was related to arcuate fasciculus volume in consistent-left-handers only, and only in people who were not right hemisphere lateralized for language; given the small sample size for this finding, future investigation is warranted. Results suggest handedness degree may be an important variable to investigate in the context of neuroanatomical asymmetries. 相似文献
62.
Benedict RH 《CNS spectrums》2005,10(5):384-391
Cognitive dysfunction, a common feature of multiple sclerosis (MS), frequently leads to impaired activities of daily living, social skills deficits, diminished social support, and unemployment. There is growing evidence indicating that cognitive impairment is amenable to the effects of medication and behavioral counseling. Unfortunately, routine neuropsychological testing is rare in MS clinics because screening is ineffective and testing strategies are often too cumbersome or expensive. Recent research supports the reliability of a brief screening test called the Multiple Sclerosis Neuropsychological Screening Questionnaire as well as a minimal neuropsychological battery called the Minimal Assessment of Cognitive Function in Multiple Sclerosis. Data indicate that the Multiple Sclerosis Neuropsychological Screening Questionnaire has excellent split-half and test-retest reliability, and that it predicts neuropsychological deficiency with good sensitivity and specificity. Recently acquired data also show that the Minimal Assessment of Cognitive Function in Multiple Sclerosis tests have good test-retest reliability, discriminate MS patients from normal controls, and predict unemployment in MS patients. Thus, these or similar methods should be employed for the routine monitoring of cognitive functioning of MS patients. 相似文献
63.
The priority heuristic: making choices without trade-offs 总被引:9,自引:0,他引:9
Bernoulli's framework of expected utility serves as a model for various psychological processes, including motivation, moral sense, attitudes, and decision making. To account for evidence at variance with expected utility, the authors generalize the framework of fast and frugal heuristics from inferences to preferences. The priority heuristic predicts (a) the Allais paradox, (b) risk aversion for gains if probabilities are high, (c) risk seeking for gains if probabilities are low (e.g., lottery tickets), (d) risk aversion for losses if probabilities are low (e.g., buying insurance), (e) risk seeking for losses if probabilities are high, (f) the certainty effect, (g) the possibility effect, and (h) intransitivities. The authors test how accurately the heuristic predicts people's choices, compared with previously proposed heuristics and 3 modifications of expected utility theory: security-potential/aspiration theory, transfer-of-attention-exchange model, and cumulative prospect theory. 相似文献
64.
For at least 40 years, there has been a recurring argument concerning the nature of experimental amnesia, with one side arguing that amnesic treatments interfere with the formation of enduring memories and the other side arguing that these treatments interfere with the expression of memories that were effectively encoded. The argument appears to stem from a combination of (1) unclear definitions and (2) real differences in the theoretical vantages that underlie the interpretation of relevant data. Here we speak to how the field might avoid arguments that are definitional in nature and how various hypotheses fare in light of published data. Existing but often overlooked data favor very rapid (milliseconds) synaptic consolidation, with experimental amnesia reflecting, at least in part, deficits in retrieval rather than in the initial storage of information. 相似文献
65.
Perception and Emotion: How We Recognize Facial Expressions 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
Ralph Adolphs 《Current directions in psychological science》2006,15(5):222-226
ABSTRACT— Perception and emotion interact, as is borne out by studies of how people recognize emotion from facial expressions. Psychological and neurological research has elucidated the processes, and the brain structures, that participate in facial emotion recognition. Studies have shown that emotional reactions to viewing faces can be very rapid and that these reactions may, in turn, be used to judge the emotion shown in the face. Recent experiments have argued that people actively explore facial expressions in order to recognize the emotion, a mechanism that emphasizes the instrumental nature of social cognition. 相似文献
66.
Lionel Standing Ralph Norman Haber Michael Cataldo B. Dennis Sales 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1969,5(4):193-196
Seven-letter words were flashed repetitively at various durations above and below the recognition threshold for single flashes. One form of a short-term storage effect was studied by measuring the interstimulus interval between flashes at which S reported that the stimulus became phenomenally discontinuous in time. Storage times reached a maximum of about 300 msec in this task. A second form of storage effect was measured by decreasing the interstimulus interval until S could correctly recognize a repeated stimulus whose duration was below the single-flash recognition threshold; this effect extended to 900 msec in some cases. Each of these effects was reliably obtained for both rare and frequent words. Both forms of storage were increased by providing a dark rather than a lighted adapting field between presentations of the test stimulus. Implications for Information processing of brief displays are discussed. 相似文献
67.
Ronald Allan Cole Ralph Norman Haber Bruce Dennis Sales 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1973,13(1):87-92
An attempt was made to examine the manner in which consonants and vowels are coded in short-term memory. under identical recall conditions. Ss were presented with sequences of consonant-vowel digrams for serial recall. Sequences were composed of randomly presented consonants paired with/a/ or randomly presented vowels paired with /d/. Halle’s distinctive feature system was used to generate predictions concerning the frequency of intrusion errors. among phonemes. These predictions were based on the assumption that phonemes are discriminated in memory in terms of their component distinctive features, so that intrusions should most frequently occur between phonemes sharing similar distinctive features. The analysis of intrusion errors revealed that each consonant and vowel phoneme was coded m short-term memory by a particular combination of distinctive features which differed from one phoneme to another. A given phoneme was coded by the same set of distinctive features regardless of the number of syllables in the sequence. However, distinctive feature theories were not able to predict the frequency of intrusion errors for phonemes presented in the middle serial positions of a sequence with 100% accuracy. The results of the experiment support the notion that consonant and vowel phonemes are coded in a similar manner in STM and that this coding involves the retention of a specific set of distinctive features for each phoneme. 相似文献
68.
69.
A. Ralph Hakstian 《Psychometrika》1971,36(2):175-193
The oblimax, promax, maxplane, and Harris-Kaiser techniques are compared. For five data sets, of varying reliability and factorial complexity, each having a graphic oblique solution (used as criterion), solutions obtained using the four methods are evaluated on (1) hyperplane-counts, (2) agreement of obtained with graphic within-method primary factor correlations and angular separations, (3) angular separations between obtained and corresponding graphic primary axes. The methods are discussed and ranked (descending order): Harris-Kaiser, promax, oblimax, maxplane. The Harris-Kaiser procedure—independent cluster version for factorially simple data,P'P proportional to , with equamax rotations, for complex—is recommended.This paper is based upon part of the author's doctoral dissertation [Hakstian, 1969] completed at the University of Colorado. The author is greatly indebted to Dr. Gene V Glass, who, as thesis advisor, generously contributed his time, erudition, and encouragement. 相似文献
70.