全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3404篇 |
免费 | 34篇 |
专业分类
3438篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 34篇 |
2023年 | 40篇 |
2022年 | 31篇 |
2021年 | 60篇 |
2020年 | 68篇 |
2019年 | 115篇 |
2018年 | 125篇 |
2017年 | 131篇 |
2016年 | 146篇 |
2015年 | 111篇 |
2014年 | 104篇 |
2013年 | 369篇 |
2012年 | 199篇 |
2011年 | 195篇 |
2010年 | 114篇 |
2009年 | 90篇 |
2008年 | 146篇 |
2007年 | 133篇 |
2006年 | 102篇 |
2005年 | 133篇 |
2004年 | 120篇 |
2003年 | 112篇 |
2002年 | 84篇 |
2001年 | 57篇 |
2000年 | 56篇 |
1999年 | 40篇 |
1998年 | 28篇 |
1997年 | 21篇 |
1996年 | 27篇 |
1995年 | 19篇 |
1994年 | 10篇 |
1993年 | 22篇 |
1992年 | 24篇 |
1991年 | 28篇 |
1990年 | 33篇 |
1989年 | 18篇 |
1988年 | 28篇 |
1987年 | 21篇 |
1986年 | 25篇 |
1985年 | 20篇 |
1984年 | 26篇 |
1983年 | 20篇 |
1982年 | 10篇 |
1979年 | 12篇 |
1978年 | 12篇 |
1977年 | 14篇 |
1974年 | 9篇 |
1973年 | 9篇 |
1968年 | 8篇 |
1966年 | 7篇 |
排序方式: 共有3438条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
971.
Associative accounts of goal-directed action, developed in the fields of human ideomotor action and that of animal learning,
can capture cognitive belief-desire psychology of human decision-making. Whereas outcome-response accounts can account for
the fact that the thought of a goal can call to mind the action that has previously procured this goal, response-outcome accounts
capture decision-making processes that start out with the consideration of possible response alternatives followed only in
the second instance by evaluation of their consequences. We argue that while the outcome-response mechanism plays a crucial
role in response priming effects, the response-outcome mechanism is particularly important for action selection on the basis
of current needs and desires. We therefore develop an integrative account that encapsulates these two routes of action selection
within the framework of the associative-cybernetic model. This model has the additional benefit of providing mechanisms for
the incentive modulation of goal-directed action and for the development of behavioural autonomy, and therefore provides a
promising account of the multi-faceted process of animal as well as human instrumental decision-making.
相似文献
Sanne de WitEmail: |
972.
David A. Weisenhorn Laura M. Frey Judy van de Venne Julie Cerel 《Journal of child and family studies》2017,26(1):161-167
Alarming numbers of military veterans end their lives each day which has a profound effect on the military population. This study examines a sample (N?=?434) of suicide-exposed (i.e., personally knowing someone who has died by suicide) veterans to determine whether the proportion of individuals who report high impact from suicide exposure and those who have clinically-significant posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms is related to marital status. Using Hill’s (Social Casework 49: 139–150, 1958) ABC-X model for conceptualizing the relationships between variables, mean scores for posttraumatic stress disorder were significantly different between groups, and the odds of a married veteran reporting high-impact suicide exposure were 2.19 times lower than the odds of a single veteran reporting high-impact. Likewise, the odds of a veteran with high-impact suicide exposure having clinically-significant posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms were 10.04 times higher than veterans with low-impact suicide exposure. Findings indicate that veterans who are married are less likely to be highly-affected by another individual’s suicide. Thus, marriage is a protective factor for suicide-exposed veterans. 相似文献
973.
Behrang Keshavarz Jennifer L. Campos Patricia R. DeLucia Daniel Oberfeld 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》2017,79(3):929-944
Estimating time to contact (TTC) involves multiple sensory systems, including vision and audition. Previous findings suggested that the ratio of an object’s instantaneous optical size/sound intensity to its instantaneous rate of change in optical size/sound intensity (τ) drives TTC judgments. Other evidence has shown that heuristic-based cues are used, including final optical size or final sound pressure level. Most previous studies have used decontextualized and unfamiliar stimuli (e.g., geometric shapes on a blank background). Here we evaluated TTC estimates by using a traffic scene with an approaching vehicle to evaluate the weights of visual and auditory TTC cues under more realistic conditions. Younger (18–39 years) and older (65+ years) participants made TTC estimates in three sensory conditions: visual-only, auditory-only, and audio–visual. Stimuli were presented within an immersive virtual-reality environment, and cue weights were calculated for both visual cues (e.g., visual τ, final optical size) and auditory cues (e.g., auditory τ, final sound pressure level). The results demonstrated the use of visual τ as well as heuristic cues in the visual-only condition. TTC estimates in the auditory-only condition, however, were primarily based on an auditory heuristic cue (final sound pressure level), rather than on auditory τ. In the audio–visual condition, the visual cues dominated overall, with the highest weight being assigned to visual τ by younger adults, and a more equal weighting of visual τ and heuristic cues in older adults. Overall, better characterizing the effects of combined sensory inputs, stimulus characteristics, and age on the cues used to estimate TTC will provide important insights into how these factors may affect everyday behavior. 相似文献
974.
975.
Margreet Visser Catrin Finkenauer Kim Schoemaker Esther Kluwer Rachel van der Rijken Justine van Lawick Hans Bom J. Clasien de Schipper Francien Lamers-Winkelman 《Journal of child and family studies》2017,26(11):3055-3066
The relation between divorce, co-parenting conflicts, and children’s adjustment problems has been well established. An unresolved question for research and clinical interventions, however, is how conflicts between parents are maintained and/or escalate. This cross-sectional research tested the hypothesis that co-parenting conflicts in divorced couples are associated with perceived social network disapproval and that this relation is mediated by parents’ tendency to forgive each other. In Study 1, a convenience sample of 136 divorced parents recruited via online forums, we showed that perceived social network disapproval was indeed positively related to co-parenting conflicts and that parents’ tendency to forgive the other parent—albeit partly—explained this relationship. Strength of our research is that in Study 2, 110 parents referred to children’s mental health care because the wellbeing of the children was severely compromised by the severity of the conflicts between parents, we replicated these results. In both studies perceived social network disapproval and co-parenting conflicts were positively related and this link was mediated by forgiveness: perceived social network disapproval was negatively related to forgiveness, which in turn was negatively related to more parental conflicts. 相似文献
976.
Melissa K. Cousino Rebecca Hazen Katherine Leigh Josie Kelly Laschinger Peter de Blank H. Gerry Taylor 《Journal of clinical psychology in medical settings》2017,24(3-4):279-288
This study examines illness-specific family burden as a mediator of the association between late effects of childhood cancer and survivors’ emotional and behavioral outcomes. Childhood cancer survivors (n?=?65; ages 10–17) two or more years off-treatment completed measures assessing internalizing and PTSD symptoms. Parents reported on illness-specific family burden, late effects severity, and survivor internalizing/externalizing problems. Providers documented the number of late effects. Illness-specific family burden was correlated with provider-reported late effects (r?=?.29, p?<?.05) and parent report of severe late effects (r?=?.56, p?<?.01). Results supported an indirect effect of illness-specific family burden on number of late effects and parent-reported survivor internalizing problems, p?<?.05. Indirect effects were not found in models predicting PTSD and externalizing problems. Illness-specific family burden is an important intervention target for reducing internalizing problems in childhood cancer survivors with late effects. 相似文献
977.
978.
Dato N. M. de Gruijter 《Psychometrika》1984,49(2):269-272
In maximum likelihood estimation the standard error of the location parameter of the three parameter logistic model can be large, due to inaccurate estimation of the lower asymptote. Thissen and Wainer who demonstrated this effect, suggested that the introduction of a prior distribution for the lower asymptote might alleviate the problems. Here it is demonstrated in some detail that the standard error of the location parameter can be made acceptably small in this way.The author thanks Pieter Vijn for his helpful comments. 相似文献
979.
Nancy S. Hemmes Bruce L. Brown James J. Jakubow Soledad Cabeza de Vaca 《Learning and motivation》1997,28(4):542-557
Elimination of autoshaped responding by random or negatively-contingent response-reductive procedures may leave a previously acquired association intact; however, two previous studies suggest that response elimination by backward conditioning may have more permanent effects. In Experiments 1 and 3, the autoshaped responding of pigeons was eliminated by a backward or negative contingency. Although speed of recovery during a subsequent extinction phase was greater following the backward contingency in Experiment 3, level of recovery did not differ as a function of response elimination procedure in either experiment. A possible basis for the discrepancy between the present and previous findings is the contingency arranged between the conditioned and unconditioned stimuli (CS and US, respectively) during forward conditioning. In Experiments 1 and 2, probability of US presentation following the CS (p(US|CS) = 0.4 or 1.0) was varied within (Experiment 1) or between (Experiment 2) groups of birds. In both cases, level of recovery was higher following training under (p(US|CS) = 0.4). The results are consistent with a memory retrieval theory in which unreinforced trials function as a retrieval cue for not responding. 相似文献
980.
Contextual control of emergent equivalence relations. 总被引:1,自引:8,他引:1