全文获取类型
收费全文 | 224篇 |
免费 | 11篇 |
专业分类
235篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 3篇 |
2018年 | 8篇 |
2017年 | 6篇 |
2016年 | 6篇 |
2015年 | 5篇 |
2014年 | 4篇 |
2013年 | 19篇 |
2012年 | 8篇 |
2011年 | 8篇 |
2010年 | 2篇 |
2009年 | 13篇 |
2008年 | 8篇 |
2007年 | 12篇 |
2006年 | 4篇 |
2005年 | 11篇 |
2004年 | 3篇 |
2003年 | 4篇 |
2002年 | 6篇 |
2001年 | 7篇 |
2000年 | 5篇 |
1998年 | 6篇 |
1997年 | 3篇 |
1996年 | 2篇 |
1995年 | 2篇 |
1994年 | 3篇 |
1992年 | 5篇 |
1990年 | 5篇 |
1988年 | 3篇 |
1987年 | 3篇 |
1986年 | 5篇 |
1985年 | 3篇 |
1983年 | 4篇 |
1982年 | 3篇 |
1979年 | 3篇 |
1977年 | 2篇 |
1976年 | 4篇 |
1975年 | 2篇 |
1974年 | 3篇 |
1972年 | 4篇 |
1970年 | 3篇 |
1968年 | 3篇 |
1967年 | 2篇 |
1965年 | 1篇 |
1963年 | 1篇 |
1960年 | 1篇 |
1953年 | 1篇 |
1952年 | 1篇 |
1951年 | 1篇 |
1950年 | 2篇 |
1949年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有235条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
41.
Psychological research shows that learning can be powerfully enhanced through testing, but this finding has so far been confined
to memory tasks requiring verbal responses. We explored whether testing can enhance learning of visuospatial information in
maps. Fifty subjects each studied two maps, one through conventional study, and the other through computer-prompted tests.
For the tests, the subjects were repeatedly presented with the same map with one feature deleted (e.g., a road or a river),
and they tried to covertly recall the missing feature and its location. Subjects’ map drawings after 30 min were significantly
better for maps learned through tests in comparison with maps learned through the same amount of time devoted to conventional
study. These results suggest that the testing effect is not limited to the types of memory that require discrete, verbal responses,
and that utilizing covert retrievals may allow the effect to be extended to a variety of complex, nonverbal learning tasks. 相似文献
42.
Our research on learning enhancement has been focusing on the consequences for learning and forgetting of some of the more obvious and concrete choices that arise in instruction, including questions such as these: How does spacing of practice affect retention of information over significant retention intervals (up to 1 year)? Do spacing effects generalize beyond recall of verbal materials? Is feedback needed to promote learning, and must it be immediate? Although retrieval practice has been found to enhance learning in comparison with additional study, does it actually reduce the rate of forgetting? Can retrieval practice effects be extended to nonverbal materials? We suggest that as we begin to find answers to these questions, it should become possible for cognitive psychology to offer nonobvious advice that can be applied in a variety of instructional contexts to facilitate learning and reduce forgetting. 相似文献
43.
Human infants imitate others' actions 'rationally': they copy a demonstrator's action when that action is freely chosen, but less when it is forced by some constraint (Gergely, Bekkering & Király, 2002). We investigated whether enculturated chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) also imitate rationally. Using Gergely and colleagues' (2002) basic procedure, a human demonstrator operated each of six apparatuses using an unusual body part (he pressed it with his forehead or foot, or sat on it). In the Hands Free condition he used this unusual means even though his hands were free, suggesting a free choice. In the Hands Occupied condition he used the unusual means only because his hands were occupied, suggesting a constrained or forced choice. Like human infants, chimpanzees imitated the modeled action more often in the Hands Free than in the Hands Occupied condition. Enculturated chimpanzees thus have some understanding of the rationality of others' intentional actions, and use this understanding when imitating others. 相似文献
44.
Kristin Liebal Tanya Behne Malinda Carpenter Michael Tomasello 《Developmental science》2009,12(2):264-271
We investigated whether 1‐year‐old infants use their shared experience with an adult to determine the meaning of a pointing gesture. In the first study, after two adults had each shared a different activity with the infant, one of the adults pointed to a target object. Eighteen‐ but not 14‐month‐olds responded appropriately to the pointing gesture based on the particular activity they had previously shared with that particular adult. In the second study, 14‐month‐olds were successful in a simpler procedure in which the pointing adult either had or had not shared a relevant activity with the infant prior to the pointing. Infants just beginning to learn language thus already show a complex understanding of the pragmatics of cooperative communication in which shared experience with particular individuals plays a crucial role. 相似文献
45.
46.
We propose a model that explains how the working-memory capacity of a comprehender can constrain syntactic parsing and thereby affect the processing of syntactic ambiguities. The model's predictions are examined in four experiments that measure the reading times for two constructions that contain a temporary syntactic ambiguity. An example of the syntactic ambiguity is The soldiers warned about the dangers . . . ; the verb warned may either be the main verb, in which case soldiers is the agent; or the verb warned may introduce a relative clause, in which case soldiers is the patient of warned rather than the agent, as in The soldiers warned about the dangers conducted the midnight raid. The model proposes that both alternative interpretations of warned are initially activated. However, the duration for which both interpretations are maintained depends, in part, on the reader's working-memory capacity, which can be assessed by the Reading Span task (Daneman & Carpenter, 1980). The word-by-word reading times indicate that all subjects do additional processing after encountering an ambiguity, suggesting that they generate both representations. Furthermore, readers with larger working-memory capacities maintain both representations for some period of time (several words), whereas readers with smaller working-memory capacities revert to maintaining only the more likely representation. 相似文献
47.
Murray E. Jarvik Martin R. Rosenblatt Catherine L. Carpenter Nicholas H. Caskey Todd M. Gross William J. McCarthy 《Behavior research methods》1992,24(3):420-422
A method and an apparatus are described for measuring the point at which subjects elect to stop smoking when cigarette smoke is delivered more rapidly than in typical smoking. The device allows either the subject or the experimenter to control the volume of smoke and the interval between puffs. The device delivers reliable quantities of particulate matter from cigarette smoke. Subjects retested a week apart exhibited consistent stopping points. 相似文献
48.
D L Hamilton P D Grubb D A Acorn T K Trolier S Carpenter 《Journal of personality and social psychology》1990,59(5):891-898
This research compared the processing and retrieval of attribution-relevant information when the attributional inference is easy or difficult to make. Subjects attributed behavioral events to the person or to the situation, based on several items of context information. Each context sentence implied either the person or the entity as causal agent. When the attributional inference was difficult to make (an equal number of context sentences implied actor and entity as the causal agent), subjects recalled more of the behavioral events, recalled more context sentences, and were less confident in their attributions than when the attributional inference was easy to make (most context sentences implied the same causal agent). Subjects also recalled context information that was implicationally incongruent with the majority of the other context sentences with a higher probability than when that same information was implicationally congruent. 相似文献
49.
50.
Two studies examined mother's labeling of gender-neutral characters in young children's picture books. In the first study, mothers and their 18- to 38-month-old children looked at three popular children's books together. The mothers' use of masculine or feminine names or pronouns to refer to gender-neutral characters was recorded. The data revealed an extreme masculine bias: 95% of all characters of indeterminate gender were referred to by the mothers as males. In a second study, specially prepared picture books were used to examine the effect of gender-relevant variables on the mothers' labeling. The results replicated Study 1 in showing a strong bias toward referring to neutral pictures with masculine labels: The incidence of masculine labels was again high and, in addition, was relatively impervious to most of the gender-relevant manipulations included in the books. The one variable that had a strong, consistent effect on the mothers' labeling was the presence of an adult in the pictures: Child Characters were nearly always referred to as males when they appeared alone; they were more likely to be given feminine or neutral lables when they were pictured in the presence of an adult. This result is related to Carpenter's [Activity Structure and Play: Implications for Socialization, in M. Liss (Ed.), Social and Cognitive Skills: Sex Roles and Childrens Play, New York: Academic Press, 1983] model of the development of sex-typed behavior. The data are also discussed with respect to the well-documented sexist bias in picture books for preschool children.This research was support in part by Grants HD-05951 from NICHHD and R01 MH33082-01 from NIMH to the first and third authors, respectively. We wish to thank Linda Roots and Elizabeth Lewis for their assitance with data analyses. 相似文献