全文获取类型
收费全文 | 887篇 |
免费 | 10篇 |
专业分类
897篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 3篇 |
2022年 | 4篇 |
2021年 | 14篇 |
2020年 | 10篇 |
2019年 | 21篇 |
2018年 | 11篇 |
2017年 | 21篇 |
2016年 | 24篇 |
2015年 | 18篇 |
2014年 | 27篇 |
2013年 | 86篇 |
2012年 | 50篇 |
2011年 | 46篇 |
2010年 | 24篇 |
2009年 | 26篇 |
2008年 | 26篇 |
2007年 | 38篇 |
2006年 | 37篇 |
2005年 | 33篇 |
2004年 | 34篇 |
2003年 | 25篇 |
2002年 | 29篇 |
2001年 | 13篇 |
2000年 | 13篇 |
1999年 | 21篇 |
1998年 | 21篇 |
1997年 | 13篇 |
1996年 | 7篇 |
1995年 | 5篇 |
1994年 | 14篇 |
1993年 | 14篇 |
1992年 | 14篇 |
1991年 | 9篇 |
1990年 | 11篇 |
1989年 | 15篇 |
1988年 | 8篇 |
1987年 | 11篇 |
1986年 | 11篇 |
1985年 | 11篇 |
1984年 | 6篇 |
1983年 | 13篇 |
1982年 | 10篇 |
1981年 | 6篇 |
1980年 | 9篇 |
1979年 | 6篇 |
1977年 | 6篇 |
1976年 | 8篇 |
1974年 | 4篇 |
1973年 | 2篇 |
1968年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有897条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
171.
We used visual search to explore whether attention could be guided by Kanizsa-type subjective contours and by subjective contours induced by line ends. Unlike in previous experiments, we compared search performance with subjective contours against performance with real, luminance contours, and we had observers search for orientations or shapes produced by subjective contours, rather than searching for the presence of the contours themselves. Visual search for one orientation or shape among distractors of another orientation or shape was efficient when the items were defined by luminance contours. Search was much less efficient among items defined by Kanizsa-type subjective contours. Search remained efficient when the items were defined by subjective contours induced by line ends. The difference between Kanizsa-type subjective contour and subjective contours induced by line ends is consistent with physiological evidence suggesting that the brain mechanisms underlying the perception of these two kinds of subjective contours may be different. 相似文献
172.
Carolyn S. Henry Linda C. Robinson Rachel A. Neal Erron L. Huey 《Journal of child and family studies》2006,15(3):308-318
We used a systems perspective to examine relationships between adolescents’ perceptions of overall family system functioning and selected parental behaviors. Self-report questionnaire data from 160 ninth and tenth grade students were analyzed using MANCOVA and discriminant analysis. The results showed two parental behaviors, support and monitoring distinguished between types of overall family system functioning. Adolescents in balanced and moderately balanced overall family functioning reported greater parental support, while adolescents in balanced and extreme overall family functioning reported greater parental monitoring. We present the implications of our findings for parent education and assessment. 相似文献
173.
Carolyn E Cutrona Philip A Shaffer Kristin A Wesner Kelli A Gardner 《Journal of family psychology》2007,21(4):754-758
Partner sensitivity is an important antecedent of both intimacy (H. T. Reis & P. Shaver, 1988) and attachment (M. D. S. Ainsworth, 1989). On the basis of the optimal matching model of social support (C. E. Cutrona & D. Russell, 1990), support behaviors that "matched" the support goals of the stressed individual were predicted to lead to the perception of partner sensitivity. Predictions were tested with 59 married couples, who engaged in a videotaped self-disclosure task. Matching support was defined as the disclosure of emotions followed by emotional support or a request for information followed by informational support. Partial evidence was found for the predictions. Matching support following the disclosure of emotions was predictive of perceived partner sensitivity. Mismatched support following the disclosure of emotions predicted lower marital satisfaction, through the mediation of partner sensitivity. Matching support following a request for information was not predictive of perceived partner sensitivity, but negative partner responses (e.g., criticism or sarcasm) following a request for information negatively predicted perceptions of partner sensitivity. The importance of considering the context of support transactions is discussed. 相似文献
174.
Carolyn M. Jones 《Teaching Theology & Religion》2007,10(3):150-155
Abstract. The Wabash Center for Teaching and Learning in Theology and Religion is a place of hospitality and its staff the epitome of the “good host.” This essay explores the meaning of hospitality, including its problematic dimensions, drawing on a number of voices and texts: Jacques Derrida's Of Hospitality; Henri M. Nouwen's Reaching Out: The Three Movements of the Spiritual Life, N. Lynne Westfield's Dear Sisters: A Womanist Practice of Hospitality, Arthur Sutherland's I Was a Stranger: A Christian Theology of Hospitality, and Kathleen Norris's “Hospitality.” Beginning with the claim that hospitality is concerned with power and grace, the essay explores the relationship between hospitality and teaching, and the modes by which the Wabash Center helps teachers both find their identities and heal. 相似文献
175.
Jennifer L. Hudson Michael Gradisar Carolyn A. Schniering 《Behaviour research and therapy》2009,47(4):339-344
Childhood sleep problems have been associated with a range of adverse cognitive and academic outcomes, as well as increased impulsivity and emotional disorders such as anxiety and depression. The aim of the study was to examine subjective reports of sleep-related problems in children with anxiety disorders during school and weekend nights. Thirty-seven children with clinically-diagnosed anxiety disorders and 26 non-clinical children aged 7-12 years completed an on-line sleep diary to track sleep patterns across school nights and weekend nights. Anxious children reported going to bed significantly later (p = 0.03) and had significantly less sleep (p = 0.006) on school nights compared to non-anxious children. No significant differences in sleep onset latency, number of awakenings or time awake during the night, daytime sleepiness, or fatigue were found between the two groups. On the weekends, anxious children fell asleep quicker and were less awake during the night than on weeknights. School-aged anxiety disordered children showed a sleep pattern that differs from their non-anxious peers. Although the mean 30 min less sleep experienced by anxious children may initially seem small, the potential consequences on daytime performance from an accumulation of such a sleep deficit may be significant, and further investigation is warranted. 相似文献
176.
Eric M. Cave 《Ethical Theory and Moral Practice》2009,12(3):235-245
Among human beings, sexual pursuit takes many forms. Some forms, like courtship, are morally innocuous. Other forms, like
rape, are categorically immoral. Still other forms are provisionally immoral. Such forms of sexual pursuit involve a wrongful
element sufficient to render them wrongful on balance provided that this wrongful element is not counterbalanced by even more important competing moral considerations. Here my focus is a particular
form of provisionally immoral sexual pursuit, unsavory sexual seduction, or unsavory seduction for short.
相似文献
Eric M. CaveEmail: |
177.
178.
Varieties of size-specific visual selection 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Compared time to evaluate stimuli of varying sizes. When Ss expect an upcoming stimulus to be a certain size, response time increases with the disparity between expected and actual size. There are, however, 2 size adjustment processes, and they reflect 2 types of visual selection. In the first, a shape-specific image representation is used to separate a visual object from a superimposed distractor. These representations require the type of slow size scaling demonstrated in imagery experiments. The second size scaling process is faster and not shape-specific. At any given time the visual system is set to process information at a particular scale, and that scale can be adjusted to match an object's size. Because both selection mechanisms depend on size, they probably occur at a relatively low, spatially organized processing level. These findings lead to a new explanation for results that had been taken as evidence for attentional selection at the level of object representations. 相似文献
179.
180.
In this study we attempted to operationalize, measure, and increase happiness among people with profound disabilities. Happiness indices were defined and observed among 5 individuals. Validation measures indicated that (a) increases in happiness indices were observed when individuals were presented with most preferred stimuli relative to least preferred stimuli, (b) increases in unhappiness indices were observed when they were presented with least preferred relative to most preferred stimuli, and (c) practitioner ratings of participant happiness coincided with observed indices. Subsequently, classroom staff increased happiness indices through presentation and contingent withdrawal of activities. Results suggested that a behavioral approach can enhance happiness as one aspect of quality of life among people with profound disabilities. Research directions are offered that focus on using a behavioral approach to investigate other private events that are important among people with disabilities. 相似文献