全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1309篇 |
免费 | 20篇 |
专业分类
1329篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 12篇 |
2020年 | 10篇 |
2019年 | 18篇 |
2018年 | 34篇 |
2017年 | 30篇 |
2016年 | 23篇 |
2015年 | 26篇 |
2014年 | 16篇 |
2013年 | 113篇 |
2012年 | 47篇 |
2011年 | 50篇 |
2010年 | 28篇 |
2009年 | 38篇 |
2008年 | 41篇 |
2007年 | 53篇 |
2006年 | 48篇 |
2005年 | 45篇 |
2004年 | 48篇 |
2003年 | 45篇 |
2002年 | 49篇 |
2001年 | 22篇 |
2000年 | 25篇 |
1999年 | 25篇 |
1998年 | 16篇 |
1997年 | 21篇 |
1996年 | 20篇 |
1995年 | 29篇 |
1994年 | 25篇 |
1993年 | 19篇 |
1992年 | 24篇 |
1991年 | 11篇 |
1990年 | 22篇 |
1989年 | 13篇 |
1988年 | 15篇 |
1987年 | 15篇 |
1986年 | 15篇 |
1985年 | 18篇 |
1984年 | 19篇 |
1983年 | 19篇 |
1982年 | 13篇 |
1981年 | 19篇 |
1980年 | 21篇 |
1979年 | 17篇 |
1978年 | 18篇 |
1977年 | 18篇 |
1976年 | 6篇 |
1975年 | 14篇 |
1974年 | 12篇 |
1973年 | 8篇 |
1971年 | 4篇 |
排序方式: 共有1329条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
241.
Claire Penn Jennifer Watermeyer Carol MacDonald Colleen Moabelo 《Journal of genetic counseling》2010,19(1):9-21
With its diverse cultural and linguistic profile, South Africa provides a unique context to explore contextual influences
on the process of genetic counseling. Prior research suggests intergenerational differences regarding models of causation
which influence treatment-seeking paths. This pilot study therefore aimed to explore South African traditional beliefs regarding
common childhood genetic disorders. Three focus groups were conducted with fifteen grandmothers from different cultural backgrounds
in an urban community. Questions pertained to the role of the grandmother, traditional beliefs regarding causes of genetic
disorders, explanations of heredity, and prevention and management of genetic disorders. Results indicate a variety of cultural
explanations for causes of childhood genetic disorders. These causes can be classified into categories related to lifestyle,
behavior, social issues, culture, religion, genetic, and familial causes. Prevention and treatment issues are also highlighted.
These findings have implications for genetic counseling practice, which needs to include a greater focus on cultural issues. 相似文献
242.
Lahey BB Van Hulle CA D'Onofrio BM Rodgers JL Waldman ID 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》2008,36(6):807-823
Recent studies suggest that most of what parents know about their adolescent offspring's whereabouts and companions is the result of youth disclosure, rather than information gained through active parental monitoring. This raises the possibility that parental knowledge is spuriously correlated with youth delinquency solely because the most delinquent youth disclose the least information to parents (because they have the most to hide). We tested this spurious association hypothesis using prospective data on offspring of a nationally representative sample of US women, controlling demographic and contextual covariates. In separate analyses, greater parental knowledge of their offspring's peer associations at both 12-13 years and at 14-15 years was associated with lower odds of being in the top 1 standard deviation of youth-reported delinquency at 16-17 years, controlling for delinquency at the earlier ages. The extent to which parents set limits on activities with peers at 14-15 years did not mediate or moderate the association between parental knowledge and delinquency, but it did independently predict future delinquency among adolescents living in high-risk neighborhoods. This suggests that the association between parental knowledge and future delinquency is not solely spurious; rather parental knowledge and limit setting are both meaningful predictors of future delinquency. 相似文献
243.
Olson KR Dunham Y Dweck CS Spelke ES Banaji MR 《Journal of personality and social psychology》2008,94(5):757-776
For millennia, human beings have believed that it is morally wrong to judge others by the fortuitous or unfortunate events that befall them or by the actions of another person. Rather, an individual's own intended, deliberate actions should be the basis of his or her evaluation, reward, and punishment. In a series of studies, the authors investigated whether such rules guide the judgments of children. The first 3 studies demonstrated that children view lucky others as more likely than unlucky others to perform intentional good actions. Children similarly assess the siblings of lucky others as more likely to perform intentional good actions than the siblings of unlucky others. The next 3 studies demonstrated that children as young as 3 years believe that lucky people are nicer than unlucky people. The final 2 studies found that Japanese children also demonstrate a robust preference for the lucky and their associates. These findings are discussed in relation to M. J. Lerner's (1980) just-world theory and J. Piaget's (1932/1965) immanent-justice research and in relation to the development of intergroup attitudes. 相似文献
244.
245.
Teresa L. Kramer Ph.D. Aparna Vuppala M.D. Christopher Lamps M.D. Terri L. Miller Ph.D. Carol R. Thrush M.A. 《Journal of child and family studies》2006,15(4):377-392
Youth with emotional and behavioral disorders (EBD) are at an increased risk for school problems and negative consequences into adulthood, increasing the need for collaboration between families, school personnel and mental health providers. Current treatment guidelines emphasize the importance of information-sharing between providers and schools, yet few studies have addressed parents’ or students’ attitudes and preferences about this process or about the disclosure of mental health-related information to school professionals. Using a sample of 73 pairs of parents and their adolescents seeking outpatient treatment, this study assessed parental and adolescent attitudes about disclosure of mental health treatment information to school personnel. The majority of parents reported that the school should be informed that their adolescent was receiving counseling or medication for EBD, and that they should be the primary informant, rather than providers. By comparison, adolescents preferred more discretion about their involvement in treatment. Taken together, the study results highlight a number of implications relevant to the therapeutic relation and the process of obtaining and sharing mental health-related treatment information with various school personnel. Future research directions regarding the consultative and collaborative process with school personnel are also discussed. 相似文献
246.
Evidence for infants' internal working models of attachment 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
247.
This research focused on confidence intervals (CIs) for 10 measures of monotonic association between ordinal variables. Standard errors (SEs) were also reviewed because more than 1 formula was available per index. For 5 indices, an element of the formula used to compute an SE is given that is apparently new. CIs computed with different SEs were compared in simulations with small samples (N = 25, 50, 75, or 100) for variables with 4 or 5 categories. With N > 25, many CIs performed well. Performance was best for consistent CIs due to N. Cliff and colleagues (N. Cliff, 1996; N. Cliff & V. Charlin, 1991; J. D. Long & N. Cliff, 1997). CIs for Spearman's rank correlation were also examined: Parameter coverage was erratic and sometimes egregiously underestimated. 相似文献
248.
Zea MC Reisen CA Poppen PJ Bianchi FT Echeverry JJ 《Cultural diversity & ethnic minority psychology》2007,13(4):304-312
To test a theoretical approach to disclosure of human immunovirus (HIV) status, 301 HIV-positive Latino gay men were interviewed using Audio-CASI (Computer Assisted Self-Interview technology with Audio enhancement). Consequence theory and social influence theory were combined to create a model of antecedents of disclosure. Results provided support for both theories. Perceived positive consequences were associated with greater disclosure to casual partners, families, and friends, whereas perceived negative consequences were associated with less disclosure to friends and family members. Social influence of peer behavioral norms was predictive of disclosure to all three target groups. Gay community social influence produced different effects on disclosure of HIV-positive status: a negative association for casual partners, a positive association for close friends, and no relationship for family members. Time since diagnosis was also predictive of disclosure. 相似文献
249.
The Psychological Record - Participants were 2 typically developing children, aged 9 and 10 years, and 1 child, aged 4 years, with a reported severe speech delay. Five specific mand functions were... 相似文献
250.
This study used the Remember/Know (R/K) procedure combined with signal detection analyses to assess recognition memory in 20 elders with amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI), 10 patients with probable Alzheimer’s disease (AD) as well as matched healthy older adults. Signal detection analyses first indicated that aMCI and control participants were comparable on general recognition performance. As regards AD patients, they were impaired relative to both aMCI and healthy elders. When assessing Remember and Know responses the aMCI group showed diminished sensitivity for Remember responses but intact Know responses compared to healthy elders. In contrast, AD patients showed decreased sensitivity for both Remember and Know responses compared to control and aMCI participants. The response bias index revealed that AD patients were more liberal than aMCI and control participants when providing Know responses. On the other measures, response bias was comparable between the groups. Overall, this study indicates that the R/K procedure can characterize different aspects of recognition memory performance in persons with aMCI or AD. 相似文献