首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   2333篇
  免费   246篇
  国内免费   2篇
  2023年   39篇
  2021年   13篇
  2020年   95篇
  2019年   28篇
  2018年   39篇
  2017年   97篇
  2016年   96篇
  2015年   88篇
  2014年   83篇
  2013年   390篇
  2012年   82篇
  2011年   62篇
  2010年   66篇
  2009年   95篇
  2008年   63篇
  2007年   66篇
  2006年   72篇
  2005年   52篇
  2004年   50篇
  2003年   46篇
  2002年   51篇
  2001年   24篇
  2000年   31篇
  1999年   32篇
  1998年   25篇
  1997年   22篇
  1996年   57篇
  1995年   58篇
  1994年   42篇
  1993年   40篇
  1992年   55篇
  1991年   43篇
  1990年   52篇
  1989年   29篇
  1988年   29篇
  1987年   26篇
  1986年   26篇
  1985年   18篇
  1984年   20篇
  1983年   20篇
  1982年   20篇
  1981年   25篇
  1980年   31篇
  1979年   30篇
  1978年   36篇
  1977年   36篇
  1976年   6篇
  1975年   15篇
  1974年   12篇
  1973年   8篇
排序方式: 共有2581条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
131.
This study evaluated the effectiveness of the Primary Mental Project (PMHP), a program for early detection and prevention of school adjustment problems. Pre- and postprogram assessments were done with 215 primary-grade children seen in PMHP, usig teacher ratings of problem behaviors and competencies, and child-aide ratings of problems. School mental health professionals judged educational and behavioral changes in project children during the year. Significant across-the board improvements were found on all criterion measures. Modest intercorrelations among criterion change estimates suggested that the observed changes were due to program, rather than halo, effects. PMHP children also improved significantly more than matched, retrospective controls.  相似文献   
132.
133.
A microprocessor system can be used both to control aversive conditioning (classical, signaled and Sidman avoidance, and conflict) experiments and to acquire behavioral data in multiple subjects at a fraction of the cost of solid state systems. The software described in this paper can be used to program tones and shocks and count responses, independently, in up to 12 animals simultaneously.  相似文献   
134.
135.
In an experiment employing the symbol-element recognition task (Mohs, Wescourt, & Atkinson, 1975), subjects first learned six lists consisting of four words (elements) each. Each list was associated with a unique consonant (symbol). Subsequently, on each of a series of test trials, subjects were presented with one, two, or four symbol-element pairs. A positive response was required if all test words were correctly paired with their associated consonants and a negative response if any one test word was incorrectly paired with a consonant. Of primary concern was the way reaction time (RT) varied with number of pairs presented, the type of response required, and, on negative trials, the position of the mismatched pair in the test display. RT increased with the number of pairs presented on a trial and the increase was greater for positive than for negative trials. For negative pair trials, RT increased with the distance of the mismatched pair from the top of the test display. On negative trials in which the top pair in the test display was the mismatched pair, RT increased with the total number of pairs presented on the trial. A serial, probabilistic order of processing model is proposed to account for these results, and applications of the model to other paradigms are discussed.  相似文献   
136.
137.
138.
139.
140.
This study uses an interpretive interview method to examine the micro-processes of generational continuity and change among a group of social activists. Boszormenyi-Nagy's theory of responsibility between generations, alternative models outside the family, and choice are used to explain the instances of change. Implications for research and practice are addressed.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号