全文获取类型
收费全文 | 389篇 |
免费 | 27篇 |
专业分类
416篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 2篇 |
2023年 | 8篇 |
2022年 | 5篇 |
2021年 | 7篇 |
2020年 | 7篇 |
2019年 | 21篇 |
2018年 | 20篇 |
2017年 | 18篇 |
2016年 | 24篇 |
2015年 | 13篇 |
2014年 | 16篇 |
2013年 | 40篇 |
2012年 | 28篇 |
2011年 | 26篇 |
2010年 | 16篇 |
2009年 | 18篇 |
2008年 | 19篇 |
2007年 | 16篇 |
2006年 | 9篇 |
2005年 | 18篇 |
2004年 | 13篇 |
2003年 | 9篇 |
2002年 | 12篇 |
2001年 | 2篇 |
2000年 | 2篇 |
1999年 | 2篇 |
1998年 | 5篇 |
1997年 | 1篇 |
1996年 | 4篇 |
1995年 | 2篇 |
1994年 | 4篇 |
1992年 | 2篇 |
1990年 | 3篇 |
1989年 | 2篇 |
1988年 | 1篇 |
1987年 | 1篇 |
1986年 | 2篇 |
1985年 | 2篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1983年 | 2篇 |
1981年 | 2篇 |
1980年 | 2篇 |
1978年 | 3篇 |
1977年 | 2篇 |
1976年 | 2篇 |
1974年 | 1篇 |
1972年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有416条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
101.
A new paradigm, analogous to that used to study inhibition in visual selection (Tipper, 1985), was employed to investigate the possible role of inhibition in selection among instance names (e.g.rabbit) and superordinate names (e.g.animal) for pictured objects. Participants named two sequentially presented pictures in pairs of three types: SAME (e.g., RABBIT-RABBIT), SAME CATEGORY (e.g., HORSE-RABBIT), and DIFFERENT (e.g., BALL-RABBIT). Naming instructions were varied so that participants either used superordinate (S) or instance (I) names for both pictures in a pair (S-S or I-I; Experiment 1) or switched predictably from one type of name to the other across the two pictures (S-I or I-S; Experiment 2). The patterns of naming reaction times revealed both facilitatory and inhibitory effects and provided preliminary support for the hypothesis that selection of a target name involves inhibition of a competing distractor name. Suggestions are given for extending this new paradigm to provide further evidence on the mechanisms underlying name selection. 相似文献
102.
103.
Carla J. McCowan Reginald J. Alston 《Journal of multicultural counseling and development》1998,26(1):28-38
The purpose of this study was to examine the relationships among racial Identity, African self-consciousness, and career decidedness in 212 African American women classified as first year or senior at an historically Black university and a predominantly White university. It was hypothesized that senior women at both institutions would have higher levels of racial identity and African self-consciousness and would be more career decided than first year women. The findings provided partial support for the hypothesis. 相似文献
104.
Stevan E. Hobfoll Carla L. Dunahoo Yossef Ben-Porath Jeannine Monnier 《American journal of community psychology》1994,22(1):49-82
Examined a dual-axis model of coping that included both action (active vs. passive) and social dimensions (prosocial vs. antisocial)
of coping strategies among a combined sample of students and community residents. We developed an assessment device to represent
the model and allow investigation. Mixed support for the model and instrument were noted. Women were more prosocial than men
in their coping, but no less active. Men were more likely to use antisocial and aggressive, but less assertive coping strategies
than women. More prosocial, action coping strategies were also more likely to be related to greater sense of mastery and more
liberal gender-role orientation. Antisocial and passive strategies tended to be related to lower mastery and more traditional
gender-role orientation. Active coping was related to lower emotional distress for men and women, but both prosocial and antisocial
coping were related to greater emotional distress for men, suggesting that men may have a narrower band of beneficial coping
strategies than do women.
This research was made possible, in part, by a grant from the National Institute of Health (R01-HD24901-01) and by support
of the Kent State Applied Psychology Center, which was established through the support of the Ohio Board of Regents. 相似文献
105.
Carla J Posnansky 《Journal of experimental child psychology》1978,26(2):373-382
Both the accuracy of category-size information and its use during the retrieval of categorized materials were investigated among kindergarten (5-year-old) and third-grade (8-year-old) children. Subjects were asked for free recall of a 34-item, categorized list wherein eight categories contained varying numbers of items. Subjects recalled items under either limited-time or unlimited-time conditions. Additionally, subjects were tested under one of three instructions: they were provided with the size of each category (Informed group), they were asked to estimate the size of each category (Estimation group), or they were given standard free recall instructions. Analysis of both the amount recalled and intercategory pause times indicated that third-graders' use of category size information was spontaneous, while kindergartners used the size information only when explicitly provided with it or asked for size estimates. Also, kindergartners' estimates of category size were much more in error than those of third-graders, although both groups erred on the side of underestimation. Importantly, when recall time limitations made the use of exhaustive category search less appropriate, third-graders were more apt to modify their search strategies. 相似文献
106.
James W Pellegrino Carla Posnansky Gregg T Vesonder 《Journal of experimental child psychology》1977,24(1):86-96
Free-recall learning and organization were measured for 3rd, 7th, and 11th graders under conditions which varied the type and amount of task structure. Task structure for the input or list-presentation phase was manipulated by using a random, constant, or alphabetic item sequence across trials. Task structure for the output or recall phase was manipulated by providing standard instructions or instructions which suggested the possible use of an alphabetic ordering scheme. Recall performance and organization showed an interaction between age and both manipulations of task structure. The 3rd graders required the maximal amount of task structure (alphabetic order with alphabetic instructions) to demonstrate recall facilitation relative to the completely unstructured control condition. The 7th graders showed recall facilitation for intermediate and high levels of task structure, and the 11th graders showed recall facilitation for low, intermediate, and high levels of task structure. The results are related to current hypotheses about developmental changes in recall performance and their relationship to changes in the systematic use of organizational strategies. 相似文献
107.
Larissa L. Meijer Carla Ruis Maarten J. van der Smagt H. Chris Dijkerman 《Journal of Neuropsychology》2023,17(3):584-589
Affective touch is gentle slow stroking of the skin, which can reduce experimentally induced pain. Our participant, suffering from Parkinson's Disease and chronic pain, received 1 week of non-affective touch and 1 week of affective touch as part of a larger study. Interestingly, after 2 days of receiving affective touch, the participant started to feel less pain. After 7 days, the burning painful sensations fully disappeared. This suggest that affective touch may reduce chronic pain in clinical populations. 相似文献
108.
Eatherington Carla Jade Mongillo Paolo Lõoke Miina Marinelli Lieta 《Animal cognition》2020,23(4):711-719
Animal Cognition - Dogs are an ideal species to investigate phylogenetic and ontogenetic factors contributing to face recognition. Previous research has found that dogs can recognise their owner... 相似文献
109.
110.
The Impact of Youth Risk on Mentoring Relationship Quality: Do Mentor Characteristics Matter? 下载免费PDF全文
Elizabeth B. Raposa Jean E. Rhodes Carla Herrera 《American journal of community psychology》2016,57(3-4):320-329
Although mentoring is a widely used intervention strategy, effect sizes for at‐risk youth remain modest. Research is therefore needed to maximize the impact of mentoring for at‐risk youth who might struggle to benefit from mentoring relationships. This study tested the hypothesis that different types of youth risk would have a negative impact on mentoring relationship quality and duration and explored whether mentor characteristics exacerbated or mitigated these negative effects. Results showed that elevated environmental stress at a youth's home and/or school predicted shorter match duration, and elevated rates of youth behavioral problems, such as poor academic performance or misconduct, predicted greater youth dissatisfaction and less positive mentor perceptions of relationship quality. Mentors with greater self‐efficacy and more previous involvement with youth in their communities were able to buffer the negative effects of environmental stress on match duration. Similarly, mentors' previous involvement with youth buffered the negative effects of youth behavioral problems on mentor perceptions of relationship quality. Findings have important implications for the matching of mentors and at‐risk youth in a way that improves mentoring outcomes. 相似文献