全文获取类型
收费全文 | 963篇 |
免费 | 12篇 |
专业分类
975篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 8篇 |
2019年 | 9篇 |
2018年 | 10篇 |
2017年 | 11篇 |
2016年 | 12篇 |
2015年 | 21篇 |
2014年 | 9篇 |
2013年 | 130篇 |
2012年 | 23篇 |
2011年 | 34篇 |
2010年 | 16篇 |
2009年 | 22篇 |
2008年 | 16篇 |
2007年 | 26篇 |
2006年 | 31篇 |
2005年 | 33篇 |
2004年 | 22篇 |
2003年 | 39篇 |
2002年 | 27篇 |
2001年 | 14篇 |
2000年 | 21篇 |
1999年 | 19篇 |
1998年 | 15篇 |
1997年 | 14篇 |
1996年 | 11篇 |
1995年 | 8篇 |
1994年 | 8篇 |
1993年 | 8篇 |
1991年 | 11篇 |
1987年 | 7篇 |
1985年 | 14篇 |
1984年 | 13篇 |
1983年 | 10篇 |
1982年 | 12篇 |
1981年 | 18篇 |
1980年 | 15篇 |
1979年 | 17篇 |
1978年 | 14篇 |
1977年 | 8篇 |
1976年 | 7篇 |
1975年 | 15篇 |
1974年 | 19篇 |
1973年 | 10篇 |
1972年 | 9篇 |
1971年 | 8篇 |
1970年 | 9篇 |
1969年 | 7篇 |
1968年 | 9篇 |
1967年 | 9篇 |
1966年 | 10篇 |
排序方式: 共有975条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
101.
102.
Colin Berry Carl Grove 《The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology》1983,35(4):553-570
Four experiments are described which aim to distinguish the relative contributions of measures of semantic distance based on formal and normative criteria. Experiment I replicates a previous finding by Collins and Quillian (1969) that sentence confirmation RTs support a hierarchically-organised memory model. Experiments II and III minimised the role of syntactic processes and examined the times taken to “see the relationship” between pairs of concepts. The results also supported a hierarchical model but cast doubt upon the formal distinction between superset and property relationships. Experiment IV showed similar results using only property relationships of the “has” form. Multiple regression analyses of the data indicate that “number of intervening links” is a more consistent predictor of RT than “associative” measures of semantic relatendness and confirm strong linearity effects consistent with a hierarchical model of storage. 相似文献
103.
104.
CP Maertz 《Psychological bulletin》2012,138(5):859-864
In "Reviewing Employee Turnover: Focusing on Proximal Withdrawal States and an Expanded Criterion," Hom, Mitchell, Lee, and Griffeth (2012) brought together many of the most important content and process factors in the employee turnover literature. In this paper, I attempt to clarify the true contributions of this framework for the turnover area and at the same time explain why improved prediction is not among these contributions. I then enumerate 3 theoretically problematic aspects of the proposed framework, which limit its contribution. Finally, I suggest 3 directions that researchers should pursue in order to test and extend the framework. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved). 相似文献
105.
Dr. med. Carl Nedelmann 《Forum der Psychoanalyse》2012,28(3):265-275
Denial is of great importance for the ego functions and for the constitution of the entire ego but little of this is reflected in case reports and theoretical treatises. The reason for this probably lies in the circumstance that two further problems play a role in the issue of denial: that of reality and that of truth. According to the theory of correspondence, truth is the equivalent of reality. According to this theory, subject and object are opposed. The repudiation of the theory of correspondence has led to blindness and deafness for many things. The modern concept of truth, according to which truth is constructed intersubjectively, destroys the opposition of subject and object. The destruction is an act of denial. Although the theory of correspondence may be outmoded, it could nevertheless retain its former appreciation. ??If we stopped reading canonical philosophy books??, wrote Richard Rorty, ??we would be less aware of the forces that make us think and talk as we do?? (citation from Habermas 2008, S.?34). 相似文献
106.
We interviewed and classified 186 quitters from many jobs and organizations via a theoretically-based protocol into five decision process types. We then tested exploratory hypotheses comparing users of these types on their propensity to report certain turnover reasons and turnover shocks. “Impulsive-type quitters,” with neither a job offer in hand nor turnover plan when they quit, reported poor management, work stress, and family demands as frequent turnover reasons, with manager conflicts and family events as frequent shocks. “Comparison quitters,” who had a job offer in hand and no plan, reported pay and advancement opportunities as top reasons with information about an alternative job being the most frequent shock. “Preplanned quitters,” who had a definite plan to quit well in advance, reported relocation and life/career changes as frequent reasons, with increasing family demand, relocation, and school starting as frequent shocks. “Satisficing quitters,” who made a plan conditional on getting an acceptable job offer, reported pay, poor management, work responsibilities, and work schedules as frequent reasons. Implications of these and other findings are discussed. 相似文献
107.
With an interest in identifying the variables that constrain arm choice when reaching, the authors had 11 right-handed participants perform free-choice and assigned-limb reaches at 9 object positions. The right arm was freely selected 100% of the time when reaching to positions at 30° and 40° into right hemispace. However, the left arm was freely selected to reach to positions at -30° and -40° in left hemispace 85% of the time. A comparison between free- and assigned-limb reaching kinematics revealed that free limb selection when reaching to the farthest positions was constrained by joint amplitude requirements and the time devoted to limb deceleration. Differences between free- and assigned-arm reaches were not evident when reaching to the midline and positions of ±10°, even though the right arm was freely selected most often for these positions. Different factors contribute to limb selection as a function of distance into a specific hemispace. 相似文献
108.
109.
110.