首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   986篇
  免费   24篇
  国内免费   2篇
  2022年   8篇
  2019年   10篇
  2018年   12篇
  2017年   11篇
  2016年   13篇
  2015年   22篇
  2014年   10篇
  2013年   132篇
  2012年   24篇
  2011年   33篇
  2010年   17篇
  2009年   22篇
  2008年   15篇
  2007年   29篇
  2006年   32篇
  2005年   36篇
  2004年   22篇
  2003年   39篇
  2002年   30篇
  2001年   16篇
  2000年   22篇
  1999年   19篇
  1998年   15篇
  1997年   14篇
  1996年   11篇
  1995年   8篇
  1994年   8篇
  1993年   8篇
  1991年   12篇
  1987年   7篇
  1985年   15篇
  1984年   13篇
  1983年   10篇
  1982年   12篇
  1981年   19篇
  1980年   15篇
  1979年   17篇
  1978年   14篇
  1977年   8篇
  1976年   7篇
  1975年   15篇
  1974年   22篇
  1973年   11篇
  1972年   12篇
  1971年   7篇
  1970年   9篇
  1969年   8篇
  1968年   10篇
  1967年   10篇
  1966年   10篇
排序方式: 共有1012条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
211.
212.
Fighting responses were elicited by response-independent shocks delivered to pairs of rats. Food pellets were presented following different non-fighting responses to shock: some pairs of rats received pellets dependent upon a specific non-fighting response to shock; others received pellets dependent upon any non-fighting response to shock; and control pairs never received pellets. The mean probability of an elicited aggressive response to shock was reduced to 0.2 by food reinforcement for a specific non-fighting response, and to 0.5 by food reinforcement for any kind of non-fighting response. These values contrasted with the 0.8 probability of elicited aggression when pairs of rats received no food reinforcement. Consistent findings were obtained when treatment conditions were changed for individual pairs of rats.  相似文献   
213.
A method for calibrating neutral-density filters is described.  相似文献   
214.
A card-sorting task involved discrimination of patterns from the same equivalence set, patterns from different sets of the same size, and patterns from different sets of different sizes. Instructions, experimental conditions, and the number of different patterns to be sorted into two trays were varied in an attempt to change Ss’ strategies from whole-pattern processing to single-element processing. Instructions alone were ineffectual in preventing whole-pattern processing, and physical masks over most of the criterion stimuli (patterns used as guides in sorting) were only somewhat effective. Differences among tasks that were attributable to size of equivalence set were eliminated only by arranging the stimuli so that a whole-pattern strategy required a great deal more information processing, or so that such a strategy could not lead to the required discrimination. The results were interpreted as confirming the influence of the properties of sets of stimuli, as well as the strength of the tendency of human Ss to process and categorize patterns in their entirety. nt]mis|This research was supported in part by a University Research Council grant.  相似文献   
215.
Rate of response as a visual social stimulus   总被引:1,自引:1,他引:0       下载免费PDF全文
In Exp. 1, a high rate of responding (chain pulling) of a stimulus monkey was established as a visual positive discriminative stimulus for the operant behavior (bar pressing) of an observer monkey. The terminal performance of the observer under conditions in which a high rate of response of the stimulus monkey alternated in a variable temporal arrangement with a zero rate of response of the stimulus monkey (negative discriminative stimulus) was essentially the same as when nonbehavioral stimuli are correlated with the availability of reinforcement. By manipulating the schedule of reinforcement to change the rate of responding of the stimulus subject without changing its rate of reinforcement, Exp. 2 showed that the effective behavioral stimulus for the observer was the rate of chain pulling by the stimulus subject. A novel intermediate rate of responding by the stimulus monkey resulted in an intermediate rate (generalization) on the part of the observer during an extinction test. These experiments demonstrated that the rate of responding of one organism can function as a discriminative stimulus to control the rate of responding of another organism; and that the rate of responding is similar to other physical stimuli in terms of discrimination and generalization.  相似文献   
216.
We obtain a generalized increment threshold law that describes the relationship between the just detectable average signal increment and the noise when the threshold is stabilized, for the usual yes-no (nonorthogonal) signal format. The results are applicable to audition and vision, for arbitrary (discrete or continuous) signal and noise statistics, and arbitrary detection probabilities. The results are most useful when the noise and signal-plusnoise probability densities have the same form. Previous treatments of this problem have been restricted to Gaussian and Poisson signal and noise densities.  相似文献   
217.
Summary According to Wundt's schema of sensory affect, increases of stimulus intensity above threshold are felt as increasingly pleasant up to a peak value beyond which pleasantness falls off through indifference to increasing unpleasantness. In psychophysical studies salty and sour taste solutions follow such a biphasic curve. Bitterness is mostly increasingly unpleasant, sweetness is mostly increasingly pleasant to a maximum with little fall off with stimulus intensity. Individual differences among adult subjects reveal some — e.g., sugar dislikers — bitter likers who depart from, or attenuate, these general trends. Studies of one- to three-day-old human neonates show rejection and negative affect for sour and bitter, but only acceptance and pleasant affect for sugar.Taste preferences of animals, albeit with expections in some species, show mostly increasing aversion for acid and quinine, biphasic preference curves for salt and sugar. When post-ingestive effects are eliminated, animals show only increasing preference for sugar as concentration increases again, a departure from the Wundtian schema. Stimulus intensity, however, is only one factor influencing hedonic value which may be drastically changed by satiety, physiological need, and learning.Preparation supported in part by NSF Grant BNS78-16533  相似文献   
218.
四、区分社会和遗传影响的尝试研究 1.寄养研究 Leonard Heston用不同遗传体质而生长在同样条件下的被试,设计了一个研究。他比较了47名有精神分裂症母亲的“实验”对象,和有正常母亲的50名“控制”组对象。所有97名被试,从初生就和母亲隔离。半数实验被试和半数控制组对象出生后立刻被送到4  相似文献   
219.
The development of high blood pressure in young spontaneously hypertensive rats (SHR) is inhibited by operant conditioning motivated by chronic water deprivation. The present study showed that the inhibition of hypertension was not due to the operant conditioning,per se, but was a result of chronic water deprivation (23.5 hr schedule) imposed on the rats from five through 12 weeks of age. Blood pressures were measured directly using a carotid artery cannula and light enflurane anesthesia. The behaviorally conditioned SHR and the water-deprived SHR controls had equal blood pressures and were significantly less hypertensive than untreated or enriched environment SHR groups. The antihypertensive action of water deprivation was not detected by weekly indirect blood pressure measurements made in the awake state. Nonetheless, the chronicity of the deprivation-induced inhibition of hypertension was confirmed by a lesser degree of left ventricular hypertrophy in the deprived SHR relative to the nondeprived SHR. Our behavioral results again demonstrated hyperreactivity in the SHR relative to the Wistar Kyoto (WKY). This behavioral hyperreactivity in the SHR may explain the exaggerated increase in drinking in the deprived SHR groups when returned toad lib conditions. The data of this study and our previous work suggest that arousal differences between SHR and WKY strains are more reliably differentiated by fixed interval schedules of reinforcement than by a fixed ratio schedule. Hyperarousal may precede hypertension in the SHR, but it is undetermined whether hypertension can be found in the absence of hyperarousal in these rats.  相似文献   
220.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号