全文获取类型
收费全文 | 669篇 |
免费 | 9篇 |
专业分类
678篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 9篇 |
2021年 | 4篇 |
2020年 | 11篇 |
2019年 | 13篇 |
2018年 | 16篇 |
2017年 | 23篇 |
2016年 | 20篇 |
2015年 | 22篇 |
2014年 | 16篇 |
2013年 | 59篇 |
2012年 | 16篇 |
2011年 | 25篇 |
2010年 | 23篇 |
2009年 | 16篇 |
2008年 | 18篇 |
2007年 | 27篇 |
2006年 | 26篇 |
2005年 | 15篇 |
2004年 | 22篇 |
2003年 | 16篇 |
2002年 | 11篇 |
2001年 | 15篇 |
2000年 | 18篇 |
1999年 | 12篇 |
1998年 | 10篇 |
1997年 | 4篇 |
1996年 | 8篇 |
1994年 | 10篇 |
1992年 | 16篇 |
1991年 | 10篇 |
1990年 | 12篇 |
1988年 | 10篇 |
1987年 | 13篇 |
1986年 | 8篇 |
1985年 | 10篇 |
1984年 | 6篇 |
1983年 | 6篇 |
1981年 | 3篇 |
1979年 | 11篇 |
1978年 | 8篇 |
1977年 | 3篇 |
1976年 | 6篇 |
1975年 | 8篇 |
1974年 | 9篇 |
1973年 | 7篇 |
1971年 | 9篇 |
1969年 | 4篇 |
1968年 | 9篇 |
1967年 | 4篇 |
1966年 | 4篇 |
排序方式: 共有678条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
591.
Color preference and food choice among children 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
The effects of color on children's food choices were investigated. Subjects were 120 children who were equally distributed among each of the combinations of age (5 vs. 9 years old), sex (male vs. female), food type (3 types of candies), and color (red, green, yellow, and orange) in a counterbalanced, factorial, analysis-of-variance design with repeated measures on subjects. A significant main effect for color indicated that children preferred foods that were red, green, orange, and yellow, in that order. Interpretation of this main effect was not interfered with by main effects or interaction terms involving age, sex, or food type, which served as alternative rival hypotheses. 相似文献
592.
These experiments evaluated two potential sources of developmental changes in pattern encoding: advances at a perceptual level enabling better representation of the spatial relations among elements, and acquisition of metaperceptual knowledge supporting a deliberate search for distinguishing features. Children 6, 10, and 12 years old, as well as adults encoded high level distortions of random dot configurations. These materials were originally used by Posner and Keele (1968). In the first experiment, subjects matched exemplars to their prototypes. In two other experiments, subjects learned to categorize distortions under two different training conditions—one designed to focus attention on individual exemplars, the other designed to facilitate comparison of exemplars within and across categories. Following training, subjects classified new instances into the learned categories. The same pattern of developmental change was found in the matching task and in the classification task: major gains between ages 6 and 12 and continued gains to adulthood. Several aspects of the results identify change at a perceptual level after age 10 as a source of this development, independent of possible contributions from metaperceptual advances. 相似文献
593.
594.
595.
596.
Benjamin?M.?Walsh Junghyun??Lee Jaclyn?M.?Jensen Alyssa?K.?McGonagle Al-Karim?Samnani 《Journal of business and psychology》2018,33(4):495-508
Scholars have called for research on the antecedents of mistreatment in organizations such as workplace incivility, as well as the theoretical mechanisms that explain their linkage. To address this call, the present study draws upon social information processing and social cognitive theories to investigate the relationship between positive leader behaviors—those associated with charismatic leadership and ethical leadership—and workers’ experiences of workplace incivility through their perceptions of norms for respect. Relationships were separately examined in two field studies using multi-source data (employees and coworkers in study 1, employees and supervisors in study 2). Results suggest that charismatic leadership (study 1) and ethical leadership (study 2) are negatively related to employee experiences of workplace incivility through employee perceptions of norms for respect. Norms for respect appear to operate as a mediating mechanism through which positive forms of leadership may negatively relate to workplace incivility. The paper concludes with a discussion of implications for organizations regarding leader behaviors that foster norms for respect and curb uncivil behaviors at work. 相似文献
597.
598.
The right and left visual fields each project to the contralateral cerebral hemispheres. The current study aimed to investigate the extent of the functional overlap of the two hemifields along the vertical meridian. We applied repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) over the left and right occipital cortex to investigate whether the foveal representation of words is bilaterally represented or is split between the two hemispheres. Employing a lateralized lexical decision task, we first showed a double dissociation between the stimulated cortical site and performance; right visual field (RVF) but not left visual field (LVF) performance was impaired when the left visual cortex was stimulated, and LVF but not RVF performance was impairred when the right visual cortex was stimulated. Unilateral stimulation also significantly impaired lexical decision latencies to centrally presented words. These findings support the suggestion that foveal representation of words is split. We discuss future strategies for the use of TMS in further tests of the split representation account. 相似文献
599.
Carey S 《The Behavioral and brain sciences》2011,34(3):113-24; discussion 124-62
A theory of conceptual development must specify the innate representational primitives, must characterize the ways in which the initial state differs from the adult state, and must characterize the processes through which one is transformed into the other. The Origin of Concepts (henceforth TOOC) defends three theses. With respect to the initial state, the innate stock of primitives is not limited to sensory, perceptual, or sensorimotor representations; rather, there are also innate conceptual representations. With respect to developmental change, conceptual development consists of episodes of qualitative change, resulting in systems of representation that are more powerful than, and sometimes incommensurable with, those from which they are built. With respect to a learning mechanism that achieves conceptual discontinuity, I offer Quinian bootstrapping. TOOC concludes with a discussion of how an understanding of conceptual development constrains a theory of concepts. 相似文献
600.
An observational study investigated the effects of gender of teacher, gender of student, and classroom subject (mathematics vs. English literature/language) on teacher–student interactions. A total of 597 high school students (294 males and 303 females) and 36 teachers (28 males and 8 females) were observed in either 18 mathematics classes or 18 literature/language classes. Students and teachers were predominately Caucasian, with the majority of students from a lower- or middle-class background. Observations were coded using the interactions for sex equity in classroom teaching (INTERSECT) observational instrument. It was found that female mathematics teachers, male literature/language teachers, and female literature/language teachers tended to interact somewhat more with male students than with female students. This tendency was not the result of male students having initiated more direct verbal interactions with teachers. 相似文献