全文获取类型
收费全文 | 26906篇 |
免费 | 67篇 |
专业分类
26973篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 47篇 |
2023年 | 50篇 |
2022年 | 49篇 |
2021年 | 78篇 |
2020年 | 108篇 |
2019年 | 169篇 |
2018年 | 3619篇 |
2017年 | 2962篇 |
2016年 | 2422篇 |
2015年 | 371篇 |
2014年 | 254篇 |
2013年 | 775篇 |
2012年 | 899篇 |
2011年 | 2686篇 |
2010年 | 2692篇 |
2009年 | 1642篇 |
2008年 | 1959篇 |
2007年 | 2445篇 |
2006年 | 262篇 |
2005年 | 453篇 |
2004年 | 393篇 |
2003年 | 351篇 |
2002年 | 280篇 |
2001年 | 149篇 |
2000年 | 159篇 |
1999年 | 124篇 |
1998年 | 111篇 |
1997年 | 90篇 |
1996年 | 96篇 |
1995年 | 60篇 |
1994年 | 54篇 |
1993年 | 65篇 |
1992年 | 76篇 |
1991年 | 72篇 |
1990年 | 90篇 |
1989年 | 54篇 |
1988年 | 56篇 |
1987年 | 54篇 |
1986年 | 59篇 |
1985年 | 60篇 |
1984年 | 75篇 |
1983年 | 55篇 |
1982年 | 60篇 |
1981年 | 44篇 |
1980年 | 38篇 |
1979年 | 46篇 |
1978年 | 32篇 |
1977年 | 42篇 |
1976年 | 34篇 |
1974年 | 25篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
91.
Two experiments examined the effectiveness of two amounts of flooding or response-prevention on hastening avoidance response extinction and on reducing CS-produced suppression of bar-pressing for food. In Experiment 1, 20 and 30 flooding trials were both shown to be effective in hastening the extinction of a well-learned shuttlebox avoidance response. In Experiment 2, rats trained under comparable conditions to those in Experiment 1 were tested following flooding for the CER in a different apparatus. The results indicated that 30, but not 20, flooding trials were sufficient to reduce the CER. In each experiment the results of additional control groups equated with the flooded groups for nonreinforced CS exposure also revealed a dissociation between the effectiveness of this CS time control procedure in hastening avoidance response extinction and in reducing the CER. Further comparisons showed that although 30 flooding trials did reduce the CER, the same total duration of nonreinforced CS Exposure in the form of avoidance extinction trials did not. Thus the context in which CS exposure occurs may affect the dynamics of extinction of the CER. The experiments are discussed in the broader context of dissociation of various indices of fear in humans. 相似文献
92.
93.
Jean-Louis Juan de Mendoza 《Psychological research》1979,40(4):415-422
Summary The aim of this work is the demonstration of a tactile-kinesthetic aftereffect. A sequence of stimuli was offered to adult subjects whose task was to compare two weights presented simultaneously to both hands by means of a Piéron gravimeter. In Experiment 1 the inspection stimuli consisted of two successive presentations of unequal weights for a period of 2 × 10 s. The test stimuli consisted of two equal weights rapidly following an inspection stimulus. Control stimuli consisting of two equal weights not preceded by an inspection stimulus were interspersed in the sequence. The results obtained confirm the existence of a contrast effect after presentation of an inspection stimulus. In Experiment 2 the inspection stimuli consisted of a single presentation of two unequal weights for a period of 20 s. The results confirm those of the first experiment and provide data enabling the contrast effect obtained to be interpreted as a Köhler-type aftereffect. In order to avoid confusion with the kinesthetic-figural aftereffect, we propose to call the effect demonstrated here the Gravimetric Aftereffect. 相似文献
94.
Susan E. Wiant 《Journal of counseling and development : JCD》1977,56(4):234-234
95.
96.
97.
The effect of incongruent color words on speed of classifying ink colors was measured in visual scanning tasks and in card sorting tasks. In both cases, little or no interference effects were noted when the classification allowed focusing on a single ink color or a set of highly similar colors (adjacent hues). Substantial interference occurred when the task required grouping of three dissimilar colors (nonadjacent hues). These findings suggest that the relative efficiency of name and visual codes in making perceptual classifications is largely dependent upon the memory requirements imposed by the task. 相似文献
98.
99.
Susan Shott 《Journal of the history of the behavioral sciences》1976,12(1):39-46
Many of the concepts central to symbolic interactionism were anticipated by the eighteenth century Scottish moralists. The symbolic-interactionist and Scottish moralist orientations both hold that society alone engenders uniquely human qualities, self arises through sympathetic interaction, and mind and self reconstruct their environments. George H. Mead's conception of thought as internal dialogue between the “I” and “me” aspects of the self and his notion of the “generalized other” were foreshadowed by some of the Scottish moralists, particularly Adam Smith. These schools differ, though, in their treatments of emotion, communication, political structures, and the origin of sympathy. 相似文献
100.