全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1038篇 |
免费 | 25篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
1064篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 11篇 |
2021年 | 8篇 |
2020年 | 24篇 |
2019年 | 18篇 |
2018年 | 19篇 |
2017年 | 37篇 |
2016年 | 49篇 |
2015年 | 33篇 |
2014年 | 26篇 |
2013年 | 88篇 |
2012年 | 45篇 |
2011年 | 32篇 |
2010年 | 26篇 |
2009年 | 32篇 |
2008年 | 36篇 |
2007年 | 36篇 |
2006年 | 22篇 |
2005年 | 28篇 |
2004年 | 31篇 |
2003年 | 30篇 |
2002年 | 31篇 |
2001年 | 31篇 |
2000年 | 24篇 |
1999年 | 24篇 |
1998年 | 16篇 |
1997年 | 11篇 |
1995年 | 13篇 |
1994年 | 16篇 |
1993年 | 10篇 |
1992年 | 13篇 |
1991年 | 12篇 |
1990年 | 12篇 |
1989年 | 8篇 |
1988年 | 9篇 |
1987年 | 10篇 |
1986年 | 12篇 |
1985年 | 13篇 |
1984年 | 11篇 |
1983年 | 9篇 |
1982年 | 13篇 |
1979年 | 6篇 |
1978年 | 8篇 |
1977年 | 8篇 |
1976年 | 6篇 |
1974年 | 9篇 |
1972年 | 6篇 |
1971年 | 9篇 |
1968年 | 11篇 |
1967年 | 6篇 |
1966年 | 13篇 |
排序方式: 共有1064条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
51.
Impulsivity has often been invoked as a proximate driver of different life-history strategies. However, conceptualisations of “impulsivity” are inconsistent and ambiguities exist regarding which facets of impulsivity are actually involved in the canalisation of reproductive strategies. Two variables commonly used to represent impulsivity were examined in relation to reproductive behaviour. Results demonstrated that sensation seeking was significantly related to strategy-based behaviour, but impulsivity (defined as a failure to deliberate) was only weakly correlated. The effect of impulsivity disappeared when sensation seeking was controlled. Sex differences emerged for sensation seeking but not impulsivity. We conclude that “impulsivity” is not a unitary trait and that clearer distinctions should be made between facets of this construct. 相似文献
52.
Mary Ann Campbell Naomi L. Doucette Sheila French 《Journal of personality assessment》2013,95(6):584-592
Scores were obtained for 103 Subjects on the Sixteen Personality Factor test, Form C; the Edwards Personal Preference Schedule; the Otis Quick Scoring Test, Form G; Raven's Progressive Matrices, Sets A-E; and on 47 handwriting variables. First, personality and intelligence variables, then handwriting variables were factored and rotated, after which factor scores were obtained. Through a regression of handwriting from personality and intelligence factor scores, it was found that 6 of 16 handwriting factors could be predicted by 5 of 10 personality and intelligence factors. These results and their relation to graphology are discussed. 相似文献
53.
Jennifer D. Tiano PhD Rebecca M. Grate BA Cheryl B. McNeil PhD 《Child & family behavior therapy》2013,35(2):110-131
Opinions of parenting programs mostly have been obtained from mothers. Because mothers and fathers' interactions with children differ, gathering data from both parents regarding behavior modification and parenting programs is necessary. This project was part of a larger study and compared mothers and fathers' acceptability of Parent-Child Interaction Therapy (PCIT) and its various components. Acceptability data were obtained using the Treatment Evaluation Inventory-Short Form and a modified version of this measure assessing specific PCIT components. The sample consisted of 40 community mother-father pairs of a young male child. Findings suggest gender differences in PCIT treatment acceptability and various PCIT components. 相似文献
54.
Exline JJ Baumeister RF Bushman BJ Campbell WK Finkel EJ 《Journal of personality and social psychology》2004,87(6):894-912
Narcissistic entitlement impedes forgiveness in ways not captured by other robust predictors (e.g., offense severity, apology, relationship closeness, religiosity, Big Five personality factors), as demonstrated in 6 studies. Narcissistic entitlement involves expectations of special treatment and preoccupation with defending one's rights. In Study 1, entitlement predicted less forgiveness and greater insistence on repayment for a past offense. Complementary results emerged from Study 2, which used hypothetical transgressions, and Study 3, which assessed broad forgiveness dispositions. Study 4 examined associations with the Big Five, and Study 5 extended the findings to a laboratory context. Study 6 demonstrated that entitlement predicted diminished increases in forgiveness over time. Taken together, these results suggest that narcissistic entitlement is a robust, distinct predictor of unforgiveness. 相似文献
55.
Three sets of meta-analyses examined gender effects on children's language use. Each set of analyses considered an aspect of speech that is considered to be gender typed: talkativeness, affiliative speech, and assertive speech. Statistically significant average effect sizes were obtained with all three language constructs. On average, girls were slightly more talkative and used more affiliative speech than did boys, whereas boys used more assertive speech than did girls. However, the average effect sizes were either negligible (talkativeness, d=0.11; assertive speech, d=0.11) or small (affiliative speech, d=0.26). Larger effect sizes were indicated for some language constructs depending on either the operational definition of the language measure, the method of recording, the child's age level, the interaction partner (adult or peer), group size, gender composition, observational setting, or type of activity. The results are interpreted in relation to social-developmental and social-constructionist approaches to gender; these views are presented as complementary--rather than competing--meta-theoretical viewpoints. 相似文献
56.
New Caledonian crows are the most proficient non-hominin tool manufacturers but the cognition behind their remarkable skills
remains largely unknown. Here we investigate if they attend to the functional properties of the tools that they routinely
use in the wild. Pandanus tools have natural barbs along one edge that enable them to function as hooking implements when
the barbs face backwards from the working tip. In experiment 1 we presented eight crows with either a non-functional (‘upside-down’)
or a functional pandanus tool in a baited hole. Four of the crows never flipped the tools. The behaviour of the four flipping
birds suggested that they had a strategy of flipping a tool when it was not working. Observations of two of the eight crows
picking up pandanus tools at feeding tables in the wild supported the lack of attention to barb direction. In experiment 2
we gave six of the eight crows a choice of either a barbed or a barbless pandanus tool. Five of the crows chose tools at random,
which further supported the findings in experiment 1 that the crows paid little or no attention to the barbs. In contrast,
a third experiment found that seven out of eight crows flipped non-functional stick tools significantly more than functional
ones. Our findings indicate that the crows do not consistently attend to the presence or orientation of barbs on pandanus
tools. Successful pandanus tool use in the wild seems to rely on behavioural strategies formed through associative learning,
including procedural knowledge about the sequence of operations required to make a successful pandanus tool.
Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (doi:) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users. 相似文献
57.
58.
59.
60.