全文获取类型
收费全文 | 16780篇 |
免费 | 249篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
17031篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 93篇 |
2021年 | 143篇 |
2020年 | 265篇 |
2019年 | 327篇 |
2018年 | 425篇 |
2017年 | 424篇 |
2016年 | 453篇 |
2015年 | 331篇 |
2014年 | 441篇 |
2013年 | 1840篇 |
2012年 | 739篇 |
2011年 | 792篇 |
2010年 | 475篇 |
2009年 | 510篇 |
2008年 | 683篇 |
2007年 | 691篇 |
2006年 | 700篇 |
2005年 | 603篇 |
2004年 | 560篇 |
2003年 | 571篇 |
2002年 | 583篇 |
2001年 | 289篇 |
2000年 | 260篇 |
1999年 | 246篇 |
1998年 | 233篇 |
1997年 | 257篇 |
1996年 | 220篇 |
1995年 | 189篇 |
1994年 | 181篇 |
1993年 | 174篇 |
1992年 | 170篇 |
1991年 | 165篇 |
1990年 | 156篇 |
1989年 | 141篇 |
1988年 | 99篇 |
1987年 | 127篇 |
1986年 | 146篇 |
1985年 | 178篇 |
1984年 | 175篇 |
1983年 | 154篇 |
1982年 | 147篇 |
1981年 | 161篇 |
1980年 | 135篇 |
1979年 | 115篇 |
1978年 | 130篇 |
1977年 | 119篇 |
1976年 | 119篇 |
1975年 | 110篇 |
1974年 | 114篇 |
1973年 | 99篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
321.
Sinnett S Costa A Soto-Faraco S 《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》2006,59(8):1425-1442
People often fail to consciously perceive visual events that are outside the focus of attention, a phenomenon referred to as inattentional blindness or IB (i.e., Mack & Rock, 1998). Here, we investigated IB for words within and across sensory modalities (visually and auditorily) in order to assess whether dividing attention across different senses has the same consequences as dividing attention within an individual sensory modality. Participants were asked to monitor a rapid stream of pictures or sounds presented concurrently with task-irrelevant words (spoken or written). A word recognition test was used to measure the processing for unattended words compared to word recognition levels after explicitly monitoring the word stream. We were able to produce high levels of IB for visually and auditorily presented words under unimodal conditions (Experiment 1) as well as under crossmodal conditions (Experiment 2). A further manipulation revealed, however, that IB is less prevalent when attention is divided across modalities than within the same modality (Experiment 3). These findings are explained in terms of the attentional load hypothesis and suggest that, contrary to some claims, attention resources are to a certain extent shared across sensory modalities. 相似文献
322.
In this randomized controlled trial, group behavior therapy (BT; n = 12) was compared to group supportive therapy (ST; n = 12) in the treatment of trichotillomania (TTM). Both treatments were also compared to a naturally occurring waiting period, the time period that participants waited for groups to form. Participants completing group BT experienced significantly greater decreases in self-reported hair-pulling symptoms and clinician-rated hair loss severity than did those in group ST. Decreases were significantly greater after treatment than after the naturalistic waiting period. In addition, a significantly higher percentage of those in the BT than ST condition were rated as much improved or very much improved on the Clinical Global Impression scale at posttreatment. However, despite substantial symptom improvement, TTM severity remained problematic at posttreatment. Specifically, few participants in either treatment met criteria for clinically significant change at posttreatment. In addition, relapse of symptoms occurred over the 6-month follow-up period. Results provided partial support for the short-term efficacy of group BT. However, the group format may not maximize the efficacy of BT for TTM. Thus, it is recommended that future BT research test either individual therapy or a combination of group and individual formats for TTM. 相似文献
323.
The influence of syllabic structure, lexical class and stress patterns of known words on the acquisition of the English stress system was investigated in ten native Thai speakers. All participants were adult learners of English with an average length of residence in the US of 1.4 years. They were asked to produce and give perceptual judgments on 40 English non-words of varying syllabic structures in noun and verb sentence frames. Results of the production data suggested that syllables with a long vowel attracted stress more often than syllables containing a short vowel and nouns received initial stress more often than verbs. Additionally, regression analyses with the three factors as predictors suggested that Thai participants’ pattern of stress assignment on non-words was significantly influenced by the stress patterns of phonologically similar real words. These results were compared and contrasted to those found in previous work with Spanish–English and Korean–English bilinguals. 相似文献
324.
The paper defends a combination of perdurantism with mereological universalism by developing semantics of temporary predications
of the sort ’some P is/was/will be (a) Q’. We argue that, in addition to the usual application of causal and other restrictions
on sortals, the grammatical form of such statements allows for rather different regimentations along three separate dimensions,
according to: (a) whether ‘P’ and ‘Q’ are being used as phase or substance sortal terms, (b) whether ‘is’, ‘was’, and ‘will
be’ are the ‘is’, ‘was’, ‘will be’ of identity or of constitution, and (c) whether ‘Q’ is being used as a subject or predicate
term. We conclude that this latitude is beneficial, as it conforms with linguistic reality (i.e., the multiple uses actually
in place) and also enables one to turn what is ordinarily perceived as a problem for universalist perdurantism viz., a commitment
to all sorts of weird and gerrymandered temporally extended entities, into an advantage, for the richness in questions allows
us to make sense of the many different readings of sentences of the same grammatical form. 相似文献
325.
Scott F. Aikin 《Human Studies》2006,29(3):317-340
Pragmatism’s naturalism is inconsistent with the phenomenological tradition’s anti-naturalism. This poses a problem for the
methodological consistency of phenomenological work in the pragmatist tradition. Solutions such as phenomenologizing naturalism
or naturalizing phenomenology have been proposed, but they fail. As a consequence, pragmatists and other naturalists must
answer the phenomenological tradition’s criticisms of naturalism. 相似文献
326.
Scott Hendricks 《Philosophical Studies》2006,129(2):317-333
The frame problem is the problem of how we selectively apply relevant knowledge to particular situations in order to generate
practical solutions. Some philosophers have thought that the frame problem can be used to rule out, or argue in favor of,
a particular theory of belief states. But this is a mistake. Sentential theories of belief are no better or worse off with
respect to the frame problem than are alternative theories of belief, most notably, the “map” theory of belief. 相似文献
327.
William D Siegfried Iain Macfarlane David B Graham Nil A Moore Patricia L Young 《Journal of Vocational Behavior》1981,18(1):30-42
Previous research has found conflicting answers to the question of whether males and females share similar preferences for various job characteristics. It is difficult to determine if the different findings are due to the different populations sampled, the various methodologies employed, or to a real change in the phenomenon itself. This study essentially replicates earlier work by having 130 male and 84 female college students rate the importance of 10 job characteristics for both themselves and members of the opposite sex. Both sexes rated motivators as important, but females also placed importance on the hygienes. Males tended to underestimate the importance of the motivators for females, while females overestimated the importance of hygienes for males. The subject's sex could be predicted by both the importance for self and importance for opposite sex ratings, contrary to the earlier study. The difference in results and interpretation was shown to be due to the type of analyses used. Additionally, it was found that females' job preferences were related to their mothers' educational achievement. 相似文献
328.
In 14 experiments, subjects had to “point to” surrounding environmental locations (targets) while imagining themselves in a particular spot facing in various directions (orientations). The spatial information was either committed to memory (cognitive maps) or directly presented on each trial in the visual or tactile modality. Reaction times (RT) indicated that orientation shifts were achieved through mental rotation in the visual task, but not in the cognitive map or tactile tasks. Further, in the latter two tasks targets were located most quickly when they were adjacent to or opposite the imagined orientation. Several explanations of this finding were tested. Various aspects of the data suggest that cognitive maps are not strictly holistic, but consist of orientation-specific representations, and—at least in part—of relational propositions specific to object pairs. 相似文献
329.
David J Hargreaves Philip M Jones Diane Martin 《Journal of experimental child psychology》1981,32(1):11-20
The “air gap” phenomenon was found to be very common in the free landscape drawings of middle and later childhood. Three experiments are reported in which the contextual cues for such drawings were systematically manipulated. When subjects were supplied with drawing sheets constructed to imply the absence of the air gap, the phenomenon was eliminated in most of their drawings. When supplied with sheets constructed to imply its presence, most subjects produced air gap drawings using contextually provided reference lines. There were no consistent transfer effects across different cuing orders. The susceptibility of the air gap phenomenon to modification by task manipulation suggests that it results from problems of production rather than from defects in the child's internal representation of the world. 相似文献
330.