全文获取类型
收费全文 | 41951篇 |
免费 | 848篇 |
国内免费 | 7篇 |
专业分类
42806篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 465篇 |
2019年 | 573篇 |
2018年 | 816篇 |
2017年 | 777篇 |
2016年 | 868篇 |
2015年 | 586篇 |
2014年 | 703篇 |
2013年 | 3141篇 |
2012年 | 1298篇 |
2011年 | 1331篇 |
2010年 | 803篇 |
2009年 | 822篇 |
2008年 | 1106篇 |
2007年 | 1107篇 |
2006年 | 987篇 |
2005年 | 845篇 |
2004年 | 829篇 |
2003年 | 749篇 |
2002年 | 778篇 |
2001年 | 1391篇 |
2000年 | 1370篇 |
1999年 | 990篇 |
1998年 | 415篇 |
1997年 | 372篇 |
1995年 | 392篇 |
1992年 | 807篇 |
1991年 | 769篇 |
1990年 | 741篇 |
1989年 | 710篇 |
1988年 | 706篇 |
1987年 | 641篇 |
1986年 | 627篇 |
1985年 | 675篇 |
1984年 | 579篇 |
1983年 | 504篇 |
1982年 | 418篇 |
1979年 | 611篇 |
1978年 | 416篇 |
1977年 | 397篇 |
1976年 | 402篇 |
1975年 | 486篇 |
1974年 | 582篇 |
1973年 | 630篇 |
1972年 | 536篇 |
1971年 | 461篇 |
1970年 | 429篇 |
1969年 | 459篇 |
1968年 | 579篇 |
1967年 | 492篇 |
1966年 | 493篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
931.
K N Brayley D J Albert 《Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983)》1977,91(2):290-299
Animals made vicious with bilateral ventromedial hypothalamic lesions had bipolar electrodes implanted unilaterally in the lateral septum, medial septum, or cingulate cortex. Four days later, the animals' reactivity and aggressiveness were evaluated 5 min before, during, and 5 min after stimulation at 20 micronA (60 Hz, sine wave). Lateral septal stimulation suppressed reactivity and aggressiveness by almost 80% compared with pre- and poststimulation levels. Stimulation of neither the cingulate cortex nor the medial septum produced a change reliably different from that seen in unstimulated control animals. Further tests with stimulation of the lateral septum at the 20-micronA level showed that neither rewarding self-stimulation nor disruption of ongoing water drinking was produced. These results are congruent with evidence from lesion studies that the lateral septum normally acts to suppress reactivity and aggressiveness in the rat; they do not support previous suggestions that the medial septum is involved in the modulation of these behaviors. 相似文献
932.
Two independently conducted studies investigated stimulus variation effects on children's discrimination learning. The stimuli in the first study were generated by a conjunctive rule and were presented in pairs. Mean trials to criterion was significantly lower for the single-feature change treatment (17.4) than for the multiple-feature change treatment (29.9). Single-feature change-treatment transfer scores were significantly higher only for stimuli that differed in terms of one of the features. The second study not only replicated the initial finding concerning the single-feature change treatment but also found that students made more errors in reaching criterion when stimuli were generated by an exclusive disjunctive rule and were presented singly. For all tasks, the single-feature change treatment with stimuli presented in pairs was accompanied by fewer errors to criterion than the other treatments. 相似文献
933.
JAMES P. SAMPSON ROBERT O. STRIPLING LARRY C. LOESCH 《Journal of Employment Counseling》1977,14(3):103-109
This study investigated seven factors relating to personal preferences in choosing a career. Differences between male and female preferences were also studied. While males and females express similarities relating to the seven factors, they also reveal differences that should be considered by counselors helping individuals explore career choices. 相似文献
934.
Richard N Aslin 《Journal of experimental child psychology》1977,23(1):133-150
Two experiments measured changes in binocular eye alignment from 1- to 6-month-old human infants. In Experiment 1 changes in binocular eye alignment were recorded from 1, 2, and 3 month olds using corneal photography. A luminous target was moved along the infant's midline at one of two constant speeds. Infants at all three ages showed some evidence of appropriate changes in binocular eye alignment (convergence as the target approached and divergence as the target receded). The likelihood of appropriate convergence and divergence increased with age, as did the ability to respond appropriately to the faster target motion. A measure of convergence lag (further decrease in interpupillary distance after target motion had ended) was obtained for trials on which the target approached the infant. Convergence lag decreased with age and was greater on trials at the faster target speed until 3 months of age. Experiment 2 measured the responses of 3, , and 6 month olds as a wedge prism was placed alternately in front of each eye. The prism displaced the image nasally (either 2.5° or 5°), creating inappropriate binocular eye alignment. A refixation eye movement in response to the prism was not present consistently until 6 months of age. Hypotheses as to the mechanism controlling infant binocular eye alignment are discussed with respect both to the present findings and to findings from human adults and animals. 相似文献
935.
Kindergarten children were given simultaneous discrimination tasks with two irrelevant dimensions varying within settings. Prior to each block of eight feed-back trials, the children were asked to attempt to provide a statement of the solution. The introtacts thus provided were found to have several of the desirable characteristics that have been reported for older children with either introtact or blank-trial probes, but which have not been found previously for kindergarten children with the blank-trial probe. Classification of the children according to the trial block on which they first verbalized the correct relevant dimension was found to account for more than 60% of the total variability in discrimination performance. The introtacts were also used to assign parameters to theoretical prediction equations, with nearly two-thirds of the total variability in discrimination performance accounted for by the theory. Extension of the method to other models is discussed. 相似文献
936.
Harrison C. White 《Journal of mathematical psychology》1977,16(2):121-134
Each partition of a population defines a homomorphic mapping of sociometric graphs onto a corresponding set of image graphs. For sets of images selected by substantive importance, the expected number of mappings is calculated when the array of sociometric graphs is drawn at random with equal probabilities. Upper and lower bounds are computed for the probability of finding at least one mapping from an array to the given images, and for the expected number of mappings given that there is at least one. Results of small Monte Carlo trials are reported. 相似文献
937.
This study examined the effect of the length of a series of movements on the recall of those movements. Subjects (n = 45) were randomly assigned to one of three experimental groups with each group recalling either three, six, or nine movements on a linear-slide apparatus. The subjects, while blindfolded, were presented with each movement by actively moving to a stop. Recall occurred 5 sec after the last movement to a stop on a trial. All subjects were given nine trials, each containing a different series of to-be-recalled movements. Results indicated that absolute error of recall of the three movements increased in a linear fashion. However, for six and nine movements, a bowing effect of the recall curve was noted following the fifth position. A primacy effect was more evident than was a recency effect. Accuracy of recall was a function of list length up to six movements, after which accuracy was not further impaired. 相似文献
938.
Analyzed 195 human figure drawings (HFDs) of adolescents, young adults, and middle-aged adults in terms of developmental differences in anxiety signs, grouped into aggressive-hostile and insecure-labile categories, and according to sex-role stereotype, as measured by the Broverman Sex-role Stereotype Scale. Adolescent males and females were significantly more likely to obtain more anxiety signs than the two adult groups, although young adults and middle-aged adults did not differ from one another in HFD performance. The most reliable sex difference was that males reveal significantly more aggressive-hostile indices in the HFDs relative to females; no significant sex differences were obtained for number of insecure-labile indices. The degree to which one has adopted a conventional sex-role stereotype was not predictive of anxiety sign differences in HFD performance for either sex or for any age group. 相似文献
939.
940.