首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   199093篇
  免费   8640篇
  国内免费   155篇
  2020年   2793篇
  2019年   3460篇
  2018年   3836篇
  2017年   4227篇
  2016年   4807篇
  2015年   3948篇
  2014年   4816篇
  2013年   23471篇
  2012年   5410篇
  2011年   4841篇
  2010年   4279篇
  2009年   4961篇
  2008年   4849篇
  2007年   4504篇
  2006年   4836篇
  2005年   4693篇
  2004年   4176篇
  2003年   3756篇
  2002年   3614篇
  2001年   4559篇
  2000年   4411篇
  1999年   3962篇
  1998年   2934篇
  1997年   2756篇
  1996年   2671篇
  1995年   2492篇
  1994年   2449篇
  1993年   2419篇
  1992年   3280篇
  1991年   3093篇
  1990年   2931篇
  1989年   2780篇
  1988年   2773篇
  1987年   2723篇
  1986年   2737篇
  1985年   2968篇
  1984年   2892篇
  1983年   2607篇
  1982年   2495篇
  1981年   2453篇
  1980年   2291篇
  1979年   2724篇
  1978年   2410篇
  1977年   2313篇
  1976年   2136篇
  1975年   2376篇
  1974年   2516篇
  1973年   2452篇
  1972年   2040篇
  1968年   1911篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
211.
212.
Are different responses differentially associable with their consequences? An overshadowing design was used to try to answer this question for three responses previously studied in golden hamsters. In Experiment I, scrabbling was rapidly suppressed by electric shock punishment, and it overshadowed a tone which occurred between scrabbling and shock. In Experiment II, no evidence of response-shock association was obtained when open rearing was the punished response, and open rearing did not overshadow the tone. Punishment had some effect on face washing, but there was no statistically significant overshadowing with this response. These results are consistent with the idea that differences in punishment suppression among these three responses have an associative basis. They also demonstrate the usefulness of a novel paradigm for studying “preparedness” of response-reinforcer associations.  相似文献   
213.
214.
215.
216.
217.
218.
Several studies have illuminated how processing manual action verbs (MaVs) affects the programming or execution of concurrent hand movements. Here, to circumvent key confounds in extant designs, we conducted the first assessment of motor–language integration during handwriting—a task in which linguistic and motoric processes are co‐substantiated. Participants copied MaVs, non‐manual action verbs, and non‐action verbs as we collected measures of motor programming and motor execution. Programming latencies were similar across conditions, but execution was faster for MaVs than for the other categories, regardless of whether word meanings were accessed implicitly or explicitly. In line with the Hand‐Action‐Network Dynamic Language Embodiment (HANDLE) model, such findings suggest that effector‐congruent verbs can prime manual movements even during highly automatized tasks in which motoric and verbal processes are naturally intertwined. Our paradigm opens new avenues for fine‐grained explorations of embodied language processes.  相似文献   
219.
220.
By roughly 6 years of age, children acquire the stereotype that men are more competent than women in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM), potentially leading to greater trust in scientific information provided by men. This study tested whether 3- to 8-year-old children differentially endorsed conflicting information about science and toys presented by male and female informants depicted as a ‘man’ and ‘woman’ (Exp1) or ‘scientists’ (Exp2). Children were expected to endorse toy testimony from gender-matched informants; thus, the key question concerned endorsement of science testimony. In Exp1 (N = 149), boys and girls showed a same-gender informant preference for toy testimony; however, girls endorsed the male informant's testimony more for science than for toys – but only when tested by a male experimenter. In Exp2 (N = 264), boys and girls showed a same-gender preference, irrespective of content. Findings suggest that STEM-related gender stereotypes might lead girls to trust scientific information presented by men over women in certain contexts.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号