全文获取类型
收费全文 | 77033篇 |
免费 | 3131篇 |
国内免费 | 49篇 |
专业分类
80213篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 856篇 |
2019年 | 1070篇 |
2018年 | 1520篇 |
2017年 | 1508篇 |
2016年 | 1615篇 |
2015年 | 1087篇 |
2014年 | 1344篇 |
2013年 | 6486篇 |
2012年 | 2437篇 |
2011年 | 2580篇 |
2010年 | 1535篇 |
2009年 | 1597篇 |
2008年 | 2307篇 |
2007年 | 2278篇 |
2006年 | 2033篇 |
2005年 | 1810篇 |
2004年 | 1791篇 |
2003年 | 1611篇 |
2002年 | 1679篇 |
2001年 | 2487篇 |
2000年 | 2428篇 |
1999年 | 1848篇 |
1998年 | 925篇 |
1997年 | 818篇 |
1996年 | 752篇 |
1995年 | 764篇 |
1993年 | 743篇 |
1992年 | 1475篇 |
1991年 | 1398篇 |
1990年 | 1365篇 |
1989年 | 1254篇 |
1988年 | 1235篇 |
1987年 | 1171篇 |
1986年 | 1178篇 |
1985年 | 1264篇 |
1984年 | 1017篇 |
1983年 | 893篇 |
1982年 | 732篇 |
1979年 | 1034篇 |
1978年 | 764篇 |
1976年 | 714篇 |
1975年 | 868篇 |
1974年 | 1001篇 |
1973年 | 983篇 |
1972年 | 875篇 |
1971年 | 765篇 |
1969年 | 718篇 |
1968年 | 904篇 |
1967年 | 859篇 |
1966年 | 775篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
991.
992.
David S. Emmerich James L. Gray Charles S. Watson David C. Tanis 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1972,11(1):65-72
Response latencies were obtained from 10 Ss in auditory signal-detection experiments. The response latencies were inversely related to certainty that a signal was (or was not) presented. The S’s decision criterion was found to have an influence on response latency, which was consistent with the hypothesis that stimuli close to the current criterion elicit longer response latencies than stimuli more distant from the criterion. Comparisons among receiver operating characteristics derived from binary decisions, from the latencies of binary decisions, and from confidence ratings show that response latencies and binary decisions together yield more information about the stimulus than does the binary decision alone. However, the increment in information gained from the measurement of response latencies is in general (though not for every S) smaller than that gained by shifting from yes-no responses to a confidence-rating procedure. 相似文献
993.
Threshold sensitivity was measured for sinusoidal movement of bright 1-deg lines against a dark background as a function of oscillation frequency and retinal location. Sensitivity was greatest in the fovea and at a frequency of 1–2 Hz. Peripheral sensitivity was more narrowly tuned than foveal sensitivity. The presence of a stationary reference line affected mainly the foveal sensitivity. The results are interpreted as evidence for both position- and velocity-sensitive mechanisms in the movement detection system. 相似文献
994.
E. C. Dalrymple-Alford 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1972,11(4):274-276
It is difficult to name the color of a stimulus when the stimulus is a word naming a different color. When the word is congruent with the color in which it is written, color naming is much quicker. Similar results are also obtained when color-related words are used instead of color names. These results are taken as evidence for the operation of associative factors that could facilitate or impair performance in the color-word task. 相似文献
995.
Rats were trained to press a lever under schedules of food postponement. In the absence of lever presses, food was delivered periodically (food-food interval). Responses initiated a second interval (response-food interval) that was reset by each additional response. Performance was first studied at different response-food intervals with the food-food interval fixed at 30 or 60 sec, or 10 min. Response-food intervals were examined in ascending order and then recovery was studied at shorter intervals. Finally, the food-food interval was manipulated with response-food interval fixed at 30 sec. At food-food intervals of 30 and 60 sec, responding first increased and then decreased as the response-food interval increased. At the 10-min food-food interval, responding decreased with increasing response-food interval. In general, very low rates of responding occurred when the response-food interval was 60 sec or more and when it equalled or exceeded the food-food interval. However, responding was maintained in one animal when the food-food interval was decreased from 120 to 15 sec with the response-food interval at 30 sec. Results, in terms of several dependent variables, are compared with data on shock avoidance. Effects of response-independent and response-produced food and shock are discussed. 相似文献
996.
Fantino E Squires N Delbrück N Peterson C 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1972,18(1):35-43
In Experiment 1, matching of relative response rates to relative rates of reinforcement was obtained in concurrent variable-interval schedules when the absolute values of the two concurrent variable-interval schedules varied from 6 sec and 12 sec to 600 sec and 1200 sec. Increases in the duration of the changeover delay, however, produced decreases in the relative response rates and, consequently, some deviation from matching. In Experiment 2, matching of relative response rates to the relative duration of the reinforcer failed to occur when the equal variable-interval schedules arranging access to the two different reinforcer durations (1.5 and 6 sec) were varied in size from concurrent variable-interval 10-sec schedules to concurrent variable-interval 600-sec schedules. 相似文献
997.
William Hodos James C. Bonbright Jr. 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1972,18(3):471-479
Pigeons were trained in a conditional discrimination procedure to discriminate between visual stimuli that varied in intensity. The magnitude of the intensity difference ranged from 0.80 to 0.05 logarithmic units. Psychometric functions were calculated from the data and the mean difference threshold for the 11 subjects was approximately 0.12 logarithmic units. A signal-detection analysis of the data suggested that stimuli that were below the calculated threshold were detectible to the subjects. 相似文献
998.
Rats pressed a lever to avoid shock on a free-operant avoidance schedule. Some subjects were also exposed to extinction in which the response-shock contingency was eliminated while the shock-shock contingency remained in effect. A specially constructed lever was used that registered not only presses, but also biting attacks on the lever. Throughout various phases of the study, shocks often elicited lever biting as well as post-shock responding. The results suggested that shock-elicited attacks that are forceful enough to activate the operandum might account for some of the responding that occurs in experiments on free-operant avoidance behavior. In particular, shock-elicited operandum attacking might account for post-shock response bursting during free-operant avoidance and the extreme persistence of responding sometimes noted when shocks are delivered during the extinction of avoidance behavior. To the extent that this is true, these phenomena should not be characterized as operant behavior in interpreting the results of experiments on free operant avoidance. 相似文献
999.
Williams BA 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1972,17(3):363-368
Pigeons were trained on a probability learning task where the overall reinforcement probability was 0.50 for each response alternative but where the momentary reinforcement probability differed and depended upon the outcome of the preceding trial. In all cases, the maximum reinforcement occurred with a “win-stay, lose-shift” response pattern. When both position and color were relevant cues, the optimal response pattern was learned when the reinforcement probability for repeating the just-reinforced response was 0.80 but not when the probability was 0.65. When only color was relevant, learning occurred much more slowly, and only for subjects trained on large fixed ratio requirements. 相似文献
1000.
Leander JD Milan MA Jasper KB Heaton KL 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1972,17(2):229-235
The vocal behavior of three Cebus monkeys was maintained by fixed-ratio schedules of response dependent reinforcement at values between fixed-ratio 1 and fixed-ratio 15. In one monkey that was exposed to variable-interval, fixed-interval, and conjunctive fixed-ratio fixed-interval schedules of reinforcement, vocal responding occurred at a low rate, but schedule-appropriate patterns were maintained. The rates and patterns of responding engendered indicated that the vocal operant can be brought under schedule control in the monkey by the use of response-dependent reinforcement. 相似文献