全文获取类型
收费全文 | 6010篇 |
免费 | 81篇 |
国内免费 | 17篇 |
专业分类
6108篇 |
出版年
2012年 | 704篇 |
2011年 | 794篇 |
2010年 | 180篇 |
2009年 | 77篇 |
2008年 | 576篇 |
2007年 | 615篇 |
2006年 | 509篇 |
2005年 | 432篇 |
2004年 | 402篇 |
2003年 | 360篇 |
2002年 | 325篇 |
2001年 | 222篇 |
2000年 | 272篇 |
1999年 | 127篇 |
1998年 | 21篇 |
1997年 | 18篇 |
1996年 | 11篇 |
1995年 | 15篇 |
1994年 | 10篇 |
1993年 | 20篇 |
1992年 | 14篇 |
1991年 | 17篇 |
1990年 | 11篇 |
1989年 | 12篇 |
1988年 | 17篇 |
1987年 | 14篇 |
1986年 | 10篇 |
1985年 | 15篇 |
1984年 | 9篇 |
1983年 | 14篇 |
1981年 | 15篇 |
1980年 | 9篇 |
1979年 | 9篇 |
1978年 | 9篇 |
1976年 | 7篇 |
1975年 | 7篇 |
1974年 | 5篇 |
1973年 | 5篇 |
1963年 | 5篇 |
1959年 | 10篇 |
1958年 | 26篇 |
1957年 | 24篇 |
1956年 | 21篇 |
1955年 | 16篇 |
1954年 | 15篇 |
1953年 | 16篇 |
1952年 | 12篇 |
1951年 | 9篇 |
1950年 | 14篇 |
1948年 | 5篇 |
排序方式: 共有6108条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
51.
Ramirez SM Glover H Ohlde C Mercer R Goodnick P Hamlin C Perez-Rivera MI 《Psychological reports》2001,88(1):189-200
The present study assessed the relationship between numbing and three associated conditions of alexithymia, apathy, and depression, utilizing data collected on 353 Vietnam combat veterans diagnosed with Posttraumatic Stress Disorder from in- and out-patient settings and an outreach center at various Department of Veterans Affairs Medical centers. All subjects completed four self-report measures: the Glover Numbing Scale, the Beck Depression Inventory, the Apathy Evaluation Scale, and the Toronto Alexithymia Scale-20. The correlation matrix indicated that scores on the four measures were moderately to highly correlated. Principal components analysis with a varimax rotation indicated a five-factor solution that provided evidence for the factorial validity of each of the constructs assessed. Results of the factor analysis of items from the four measures were consistent with numbing being a separate and distinct construct from alexithymia, apathy, and depression. In general, results indicated that all constructs measured were separate and distinct from one another. 相似文献
52.
Heyman RE Chaudhry BR Treboux D Crowell J Lord C Vivian D Waters EB 《Behavior Therapy》2001,32(1):107-122
Using three different samples of couples (clinic, nondistressed community, and engaged), we found that 15 minutes was sufficient to witness enough behavior to make reliable (i.e., internally consistent) estimations of most Rapid Marital Interaction Coding System (Heyman & Vivian, 1993) code frequencies. Ten minutes is sufficient for many codes of interest. The ease in which "how much time is necessary" calculations can be made should entice behavioral investigators from a variety of content areas to publish such figures. By empirically investigating a factor that in most fields becomes reified through convention, investigators can conduct observational research that is both maximally efficient and maximally scientifically defensible. 相似文献
53.
Cosans C 《Theoretical medicine and bioethics》2001,22(1):47-55
This paper rejects dualism between mind and body toview the self as an embodied biological entity. Rather thanseeing the body operating by passive mechanisms as Descartesargues, it holds it actively moves in and even defines its world. Carrying this perspective to medicine presents an attempt toincorporate or work with internal processes of the body; it issensitive to how patients identify with their bodies. Thecurrent discussion over the extent to which women should try tohave natural childbirths provides a concrete example of thedifferences between mechanistic and embodied approaches tomedicine. 相似文献
54.
55.
People can learn word–referent pairs over a short series of individually ambiguous situations containing multiple words and
referents (Yu & Smith, 2007, Cognition 106: 1558–1568). Cross-situational statistical learning relies on the repeated co-occurrence of words with their
intended referents, but simple co-occurrence counts cannot explain the findings. Mutual exclusivity (ME: an assumption of
one-to-one mappings) can reduce ambiguity by leveraging prior experience to restrict the number of word–referent pairings
considered but can also block learning of non-one-to-one mappings. The present study first trained learners on one-to-one
mappings with varying numbers of repetitions. In late training, a new set of word–referent pairs were introduced alongside
pretrained pairs; each pretrained pair consistently appeared with a new pair. Results indicate that (1) learners quickly infer
new pairs in late training on the basis of their knowledge of pretrained pairs, exhibiting ME; and (2) learners also adaptively
relax the ME bias and learn two-to-two mappings involving both pretrained and new words and objects. We present an associative
model that accounts for both results using competing familiarity and uncertainty biases. 相似文献
56.
When participants are asked to recall lists of items in the reverse order, known as backward recall, several benchmark memory phenomena, such as the word length effect, are abolished (Bireta et al. Memory & Cognition 38:279–291,
2010). Bireta et al. (Memory & Cognition 38:279–291, 2010) suggested that in backward recall, reliance on order retention is increased at the expense of item retention, leading to
the abolition of item-based phenomena. In a subsequent study, however, Guérard and Saint-Aubin (in press) showed that four lexical factors known to modulate item retention were unaffected by recall direction. In a series of five
experiments, we examined the source of the discrepancy between the two studies. We revisited the effects of phonological similarity,
word length, articulatory suppression, and irrelevant speech, using open and closed pools of words in backward and forward
recall. The results are unequivocal in showing that none of these effects are influenced by recall direction, suggesting that
Bireta et al.’s (Memory & Cognition 38:279–291, 2010) results are the consequence of their particular stimuli. 相似文献
57.
58.
This research program explored how the positivity of people's memories of their past personal attributes is influenced by their desire to cope with negative mood states. The studies tested the hypothesis that beliefs and motives regarding the stability of personality will determine whether people idealize or derogate their earlier attributes in an attempt to repair distressing feelings. When knowledge structures or motives implying personal change are activated, people should derogate their past selves in response to negative moods; in contrast, when these factors imply personal stability, people should idealize their past selves in response to negative moods. Studies 1-3, which assessed the impact of mood negativity (neutral vs. negative) and theories (or motives) regarding personal change (change vs. stability) on the positivity of people's memories of their past attributes, supported this reasoning. Study 4 extended these findings by examining how an underlying mediating variable--mood-repair motivation--guides the effect of negative moods on recall of past selves. Implications of the results for research on temporal comparison, mood-congruent recall, and posttraumatic growth are discussed. 相似文献
59.
Armstrong RB Damaraju C Ascher S Schwarzman L O'Neill J Starr HL 《Journal of attention disorders》2012,16(8):697-705
Objective: The authors evaluated the time course of the treatment effect of Osmotic-Release Oral System methylphenidate (OROS(?) MPH) HCl (Concerta(?), Raritan, NJ) CII in children with ADHD. Method: Data were combined from two double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled, cross-over, analog classroom studies in children (9-12 years) with ADHD. Participants received an individualized dose of placebo or OROS(?) MPH on two laboratory school days. Permanent Product Math Test and Swanson, Kotkin, Agler, M-Flynn, and Pelham scores were evaluated 0.5 hr before dosing and 1, 2, 4, 10, 11, and 12.5 hr post dose. Analysis used a repeated-measures mixed model. Results: Treatment effects were present at all postdose assessment points (p < .0001 for all comparisons, n = 139). Adverse events were similar to previous reports for OROS(?) MPH. Conclusion: A robust treatment effect occurred with OROS(?) MPH; onset was at 1 hr and persisted for at least 12.5 hr after dosing. 相似文献
60.
A recent perceptual imaging experiment uses a rare 2×2 design to dissociate selective visual attention from visual consciousness. Its conclusions support the hypothesis that visual consciousness does not arise from neurons in primary visual cortex and forces a reinterpretation of numerous prior studies. 相似文献