全文获取类型
收费全文 | 308篇 |
免费 | 15篇 |
专业分类
323篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 6篇 |
2021年 | 3篇 |
2019年 | 3篇 |
2018年 | 8篇 |
2017年 | 12篇 |
2016年 | 13篇 |
2015年 | 2篇 |
2014年 | 9篇 |
2013年 | 28篇 |
2012年 | 11篇 |
2011年 | 8篇 |
2010年 | 13篇 |
2009年 | 14篇 |
2008年 | 11篇 |
2007年 | 10篇 |
2006年 | 10篇 |
2005年 | 11篇 |
2004年 | 11篇 |
2003年 | 12篇 |
2002年 | 13篇 |
2001年 | 4篇 |
2000年 | 8篇 |
1999年 | 4篇 |
1998年 | 7篇 |
1997年 | 4篇 |
1996年 | 4篇 |
1995年 | 2篇 |
1993年 | 4篇 |
1992年 | 4篇 |
1991年 | 4篇 |
1989年 | 2篇 |
1988年 | 2篇 |
1987年 | 4篇 |
1986年 | 3篇 |
1985年 | 3篇 |
1984年 | 5篇 |
1983年 | 3篇 |
1981年 | 4篇 |
1980年 | 2篇 |
1979年 | 4篇 |
1978年 | 3篇 |
1977年 | 2篇 |
1976年 | 5篇 |
1975年 | 2篇 |
1974年 | 5篇 |
1973年 | 4篇 |
1971年 | 2篇 |
1968年 | 2篇 |
1967年 | 2篇 |
1965年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有323条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
71.
Robin L. Peterson Anne B. Arnett Bruce F. Pennington Brian Byrne Stefan Samuelsson Richard K. Olson 《Developmental science》2018,21(3)
Previous research has established that learning to read improves children's performance on reading‐related phonological tasks, including phoneme awareness (PA) and nonword repetition. Few studies have investigated whether literacy acquisition also promotes children's rapid automatized naming (RAN). We tested the hypothesis that literacy acquisition should influence RAN in an international, longitudinal population sample of twins. Cross‐lagged path models evaluated the relationships among literacy, PA, and RAN across four time points from pre‐kindergarten through grade 4. Consistent with previous research, literacy showed bidirectional relationships with reading‐related oral language skills. We found novel evidence for an effect of earlier literacy on later RAN, which was most evident in children at early phases of literacy development. In contrast, the influence of earlier RAN on later literacy was predominant among older children. These findings imply that the association between these two related skills is moderated by development. Implications for models of reading development and for dyslexia research are discussed. 相似文献
72.
Elaborate manual skills of food processing are known in several species of great ape; but their manner of acquisition is controversial.
Local, “cultural” traditions show the influence of social learning, but it is uncertain whether this includes the ability
to imitate the organization of behavior. Dispute has centered on whether program-level imitation contributes to the acquisition
of feeding techniques in gorillas. Here, we show that captive western gorillas at Port Lympne, Kent, have developed a group-wide
habit of feeding on nettles, using two techniques. We compare their nettle processing behavior with that of wild mountain
gorillas in Rwanda. Both populations are similar in their repertoires of action elements, and in developing multi-step techniques
for food processing, with coordinated asymmetric actions of the hands and iteration of parts of a process as “subroutines”.
Crucially, however, the two populations deal in different ways with the special challenges presented by nettle stings, with
consistently different organizations of action elements. We conclude that, while an elaborate repertoire of manual actions
and the ability to develop complex manual skills are natural characteristics of gorillas, the inter-site differences in nettle-eating
technique are best explained as a consequence of social transmission. According to this explanation, gorillas can copy aspects
of program organization from the behavior of others and they use this ability when learning how to eat nettles, resulting
in consistent styles of processing by most individuals at each different site; like other great apes, gorillas have the precursor
abilities for developing culture. 相似文献
73.
Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy - Parent management training has demonstrated effectiveness in the treatment of child behavioural issues and associated conduct difficulties. Anger,... 相似文献
74.
Effects of D-amphetamine on response acquisition with immediate and delayed reinforcement. 下载免费PDF全文
The present study examined in 8-hour sessions the effects of d-amphetamine (1.0, 5.6, and 10 mg/kg) on the acquisition of lever-press responding in rats that were exposed to procedures in which water delivery was delayed by 0, 8, or 16 seconds relative to the response that produced it. Both nonresetting- and resetting-delay conditions were studied. Although neither shaping nor autoshaping occurred, substantial levels of operative-lever responding developed under all conditions in which responses produced water. The lowest dose (1.0 mg/kg) of d-amphetamine either had no effect on or increased operative-lever pressing, whereas higher doses typically produced an initial reduction in lever pressing. Nonetheless, overall rates of operative-lever pressing at these doses were as high as, or higher than, those observed with vehicle. Thus, response acquisition was observed under all reinforcement procedures at all drug doses. In the absence of the drug, most responding occurred on the operative lever when reinforcement was immediate. Such differential responding also developed under both nonresetting- and resetting-delay procedures when the delay was 8 seconds, but not when it was 16 seconds. d-Amphetamine did not affect the development of differential responding under any procedure. Thus, consistent with d-amphetamine's effects under repeated acquisition procedures, the drug had no detrimental effect on learning until doses that produced general behavioral disruption were administered. 相似文献
75.
R. W. Byrne 《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》1979,31(1):147-154
Consistent patterns of errors are found in estimations of distances and angles along urban routes, even among subjects who know the areas well. These patterns can be used to discover the organization of the knowledge we use to find our way around in everyday life.
In the first experiment, 80 undergraduates estimated by ratio scaling the walking distances between pairs of locations in St. Andrews. Routes varied independently in their location, number of major bends, and length. Relative overestimation of length was found with routes in the town centre, with routes having several major bends, and (perhaps as an experimental artifact) with short routes. In the second experiment, 30 Cambridge residents estimated the angles between pairs of roads, by drawing the configuration of roads at their junctions. The real angles were either in the range 60-70° or 110-120°. All the estimates differed little from 90°, regardless of the true magnitude of the angle.
The implications of these findings for theories of mental representation of largescale space are discussed. A model is supported in which a spatial area is represented as a “network-map”, consisting of strings of locations forming a net of paths known to be traversable, but vector distance is not preserved. 相似文献
In the first experiment, 80 undergraduates estimated by ratio scaling the walking distances between pairs of locations in St. Andrews. Routes varied independently in their location, number of major bends, and length. Relative overestimation of length was found with routes in the town centre, with routes having several major bends, and (perhaps as an experimental artifact) with short routes. In the second experiment, 30 Cambridge residents estimated the angles between pairs of roads, by drawing the configuration of roads at their junctions. The real angles were either in the range 60-70° or 110-120°. All the estimates differed little from 90°, regardless of the true magnitude of the angle.
The implications of these findings for theories of mental representation of largescale space are discussed. A model is supported in which a spatial area is represented as a “network-map”, consisting of strings of locations forming a net of paths known to be traversable, but vector distance is not preserved. 相似文献
76.
Body dissatisfaction is particularly prevalent during adolescence and has recently been linked to stress in females and males. However, prospective studies are needed to better understand the relationship between stress and body dissatisfaction. The present study investigates the direction of this association and the mediating role of self-esteem and body image importance. A sample of 298 adolescent females and males in Grades 7 to 10 (ages 12 to 17 years) were surveyed at two time points over a one-year period. Hierarchical regression analyses revealed that stress significantly predicted body dissatisfaction one year later. Furthermore, a multiple mediation analysis controlling for gender revealed a significant indirect effect in both cross-sectional and longitudinal models, indicating that stress predicts reductions in self-esteem and increases in body importance, which in turn predict body dissatisfaction. These findings suggest that stress, self-esteem, and body importance should be included in programmes aimed at improving body dissatisfaction. 相似文献
77.
78.
We compared reasoners' inferences from conditionals based on possibilities in the present or the past (e.g., "If Linda had been in Dublin then Cathy would have been in Galway") with their inferences based on facts in the present or the past (e.g., "If Linda was in Dublin then Cathy was in Galway"). We propose that people construct a richer representation of conditionals that deal with possibilities rather than facts: Their models make explicit not only the suppositional case, in which Linda is in Dublin and Cathy is in Galway, but also the presupposed case, in which Linda is not in Dublin and Cathy is not in Galway. We report the results of four experiments that corroborate this model theory. The experiments show that reasoners make more inferences from conditionals based on possibilities rather than on facts when the inferences depend on the presupposed case. The results also show that reasoners generate different situations to verify and falsify conditionals based on possibilities and facts. 相似文献
79.
80.
When people think about what might have been, they mentally undo controllable rather than uncontrollable events. We report the results of two experiments in which we examined this controllability effect in counterfactual thinking. The experiments show that the mutability of controllable events is influenced by the perceived appropriateness or inappropriateness of the events. The first experiment shows that people change inappropriate controllable actions more than appropriate controllable ones. The second experiment shows that people mutate inappropriate controllable events whether the outcome is exceptional or normal with respect to intrapersonal habitual norms, and whether the outcome is positive or negative. We discuss the implications for alternative theories of counterfactual thinking. 相似文献