首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   152篇
  免费   6篇
  2020年   2篇
  2019年   1篇
  2018年   4篇
  2017年   5篇
  2016年   9篇
  2015年   2篇
  2013年   15篇
  2012年   8篇
  2011年   9篇
  2010年   8篇
  2009年   3篇
  2008年   11篇
  2007年   1篇
  2006年   5篇
  2005年   3篇
  2004年   5篇
  2003年   5篇
  2002年   3篇
  2001年   7篇
  2000年   5篇
  1999年   3篇
  1998年   1篇
  1997年   1篇
  1996年   3篇
  1995年   1篇
  1994年   2篇
  1993年   2篇
  1992年   2篇
  1991年   3篇
  1988年   2篇
  1986年   2篇
  1985年   1篇
  1984年   2篇
  1981年   3篇
  1980年   1篇
  1978年   1篇
  1976年   1篇
  1975年   1篇
  1974年   1篇
  1973年   1篇
  1972年   1篇
  1971年   1篇
  1968年   1篇
  1966年   1篇
  1965年   2篇
  1961年   1篇
  1947年   1篇
  1944年   1篇
  1939年   1篇
  1938年   1篇
排序方式: 共有158条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
21.
22.
Recently, there has been considerable interest in applying the principles of learning theory and operant conditioning to the problems of alcoholism. One principle which may be of value is stimulus control. The principle of stimulus control would suggest that there should be some degree of similarity between the environmental circumstances in which alcoholics do their drinking and the situation where they take their first, post-treatment, drink. The principle of stimulus control also suggests that alcoholics treated with aversion therapy would tend to relapse under environmental conditions that were in some way different from their usual drinking environments.Whether we are able to observe a difference in drinking environments before and after treatment depends on whether we have selected the appropriate stimulus dimensions. The present author is in agreement with Lunde and Vogler (1970) who suggest that; types of liquor consumed; drinking associates; whether at home alone, with others or at a bar; are some of the important characteristics of an alcoholic's learning history. These stimulus conditions could be further simplified to location, type of beverage and social or non-social drinking.Since it is physically impossible to turn time backwards or to have observers follow patients everywhere they go, we must rely on self-reports of alcoholics about their drinking history. Although many clinicians have reservations about the veracity of alcoholics. Guze et al. (1963) report that 97 per cent of alcoholics in a prison population could be correctly diagnosed from their own self-report. Thus, it appears that at least when interviewed in the context of a research study where no contingencies are placed on their behavior, verbal or otherwise, self-report by alcoholics can indeed be reliable. The present study examined the relapse situation of alcoholics in terms of the location of the first drink episode, the social environment, and type of beverage consumed and compared this situation to the usual (most frequent) drinking situation prior to treatment.  相似文献   
23.
Previous research has established associations between stress and coping processes and internalizing and externalizing problems among adolescents and emerging adults. The purpose of the present study was to integrate this literature with research demonstrating the importance of self-theory—views that individuals hold about themselves and their abilities. We examined the extent to which self-theory may influence both coping processes and internalizing and externalizing problems among adolescents and emerging adults. Using moderated mediation, we tested whether coping serves as a mechanism through which self-theory of both intelligence and personality influences internalizing and externalizing problems among adolescents and emerging adults. One hundred and forty participants (adolescents aged 14–17; emerging adults aged 18–30; 60 % female; 8.5 % ethnic minority) completed questionnaires on self-theory, coping responses, and behavioral/emotional problems. Results of moderated mediation analyses were consistent with coping as a mechanism through which self-theory influences psychopathology symptoms such that more malleable beliefs are associated with increased productive coping strategies and lower levels of internalizing and externalizing problems. The current findings also indicate that these associations were significant for adolescents but not for emerging adults. Although cross-sectional, findings suggest that fostering malleable beliefs among adolescents may increase the use of productive coping strategies and positively influence adjustment.  相似文献   
24.
25.
26.
The current study provides additional information about the nomological networks of the personality dimensions associated with the triarchic conceptualization of psychopathy – boldness, meanness, and disinhibition (Patrick et al. 2009). All analyses were based on a sample of college students (N = 633 to 637; 50 % women). We evaluated correlations between measures of the triarchic constructs and measures of the facets and domains of the Five-Factor Model (FFM). We also evaluated associations between triarchic constructs and measures of self-esteem, narcissism, aggression and antisocial behavior and tested whether the triarchic measures increment measures of the FFM for predicting these variables. We replicated and extended previous studies concerning the correlates of boldness, meanness, and disinhibition. We also found evidence that measures of the triarchic dimensions incremented measures of the FFM, especially when considering self-reported antisocial behavior.  相似文献   
27.
Criminals are occasionally recorded on video committing a crime. At trial, jurors may be shown images of the culprit to determine if they match the defendant. However, several sources of bias may influence juror matching decisions. Also, even with clear video, the accuracy of defendant‐culprit matching can be relatively poor. To reduce these problems, we propose that defendant‐culprit matching be viewed as a type of forensic test. If conducted as a forensic test, defendant‐culprit matching can be improved by adding fillers and testing mock witnesses rather than the actual jurors. A Bayesian analysis of data from two experiments shows that a filler‐control test can be highly diagnostic, even when the decisions of mock witnesses are far from unanimous. However, when viewing conditions are poor, a filler‐control test may not provide much new information about whether the defendant matches the culprit. Copyright © 2017 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.  相似文献   
28.
A handful of prior adoption studies have confirmed that the cross-sectional relationship between child conduct problems and parent-child conflict at least partially originates in the shared environment. However, as the direction of causation between parenting and delinquency remains unclear, this relationship could be better explained by the adolescent's propensity to elicit conflictive parenting, a phenomenon referred to as an evocative gene-environment correlation. In the current study, the authors thus examined the prospective relationship between conduct problems and parent-child conflict in a sample of adoptive families. Participants included 672 adolescents in 405 adoptive families assessed at 2 time points roughly 4 years apart. Results indicated that parent-child conflict predicts the development of conduct problems, whereas conduct problems do not predict increases in parent-child conflict. Such findings suggest that evocative gene-environment correlations are highly unlikely to be an explanation of prior shared environmental effects during adolescence. Moreover, because the adolescents in this study do not share genes with their adoptive parents, the association between conduct problems and parent-child conflict is indicative of shared environmental mediation in particular. Implications of the findings are discussed.  相似文献   
29.
T2 in an attentional blink paradigm served as a high- or low-frequency prime word for a subsequent repeated target. Consistent with research in visual word identification, only reported primes facilitated the identification of a target repeated approximately 8 s after RSVP. Priming was greater for low- than high-frequency words. Analogous with masked priming, a blinked T2 facilitated report of a repeated target occurring 318 ms after T2 in RSVP. The blinked repetition priming effect was additive with target frequency. These results indicate that: (1) the outcomes of processing prime words are a key factor in repetition priming effects, with blinked and reported T2s behaving like masked and unmasked primes, respectively, (2) there may be different sources of repetition effects, (3) there is a consistent cross-paradigm pattern of repetition effects that occurs as a function of prime–target interval and the ability to identify the word on its first and second presentation.  相似文献   
30.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号