全文获取类型
收费全文 | 400篇 |
免费 | 5篇 |
专业分类
405篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 3篇 |
2021年 | 4篇 |
2020年 | 7篇 |
2019年 | 13篇 |
2018年 | 5篇 |
2017年 | 6篇 |
2016年 | 11篇 |
2015年 | 11篇 |
2014年 | 10篇 |
2013年 | 40篇 |
2012年 | 22篇 |
2011年 | 11篇 |
2010年 | 12篇 |
2009年 | 10篇 |
2008年 | 19篇 |
2007年 | 14篇 |
2006年 | 19篇 |
2005年 | 9篇 |
2004年 | 11篇 |
2003年 | 11篇 |
2002年 | 6篇 |
2001年 | 8篇 |
2000年 | 17篇 |
1999年 | 7篇 |
1998年 | 3篇 |
1997年 | 4篇 |
1996年 | 3篇 |
1995年 | 4篇 |
1994年 | 5篇 |
1993年 | 3篇 |
1992年 | 6篇 |
1990年 | 9篇 |
1989年 | 4篇 |
1987年 | 4篇 |
1986年 | 8篇 |
1985年 | 3篇 |
1983年 | 4篇 |
1982年 | 5篇 |
1981年 | 4篇 |
1980年 | 4篇 |
1979年 | 2篇 |
1978年 | 2篇 |
1977年 | 5篇 |
1976年 | 3篇 |
1975年 | 2篇 |
1974年 | 5篇 |
1973年 | 2篇 |
1972年 | 2篇 |
1971年 | 2篇 |
1958年 | 4篇 |
排序方式: 共有405条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
171.
It has been proposed that time perspective is an important variable that can, in part, explain the differences between people in terms of the development of self-control. One corollary of this position is that time perspective is somehow related to systematic biases in the way people perceive the passage of time. Such a bias may or may not be augmented by an association between time perspective and impulsivity. Two studies were conducted using measures of temporal extension, time estimation, and impulsivity. In general, no consistent effects were found. Neither time perspective nor impulsivity was related to any characteristic pattern of errors. In the second study, time perspective (mediated by age) was associated with predicting very brief time estimation scores, but not longer time estimation periods. It was concluded that whatever the mechanism might be that underlies the purported effect between time perspective and self-control, it is not related to a differential ability to perceive time moving more or less quickly. 相似文献
172.
Charlene P. E. Burns 《Theology & Science》2013,11(3):279-290
Theodicy has become problematic in light of scientific theories of evolution, and all theologies are doomed to failure unless and until they are “honest, and ever-vigilant against the temptation … to excise from [nature] … violence and indifference to suffering” simply because it does not suit our theological systems or political goals.1 In this theological attempt to deal directly with the violence inherent in the evolutionary process, I suggest a cautious retrieval of Martin Luther's God Hidden/God Revealed in order to allow for a more honest appraisal of suffering in this evolutionary universe. 相似文献
173.
174.
This study examined the mechanism by which stressors, dissatisfaction with family, perceived control, social support, and coping were related to psychological distress in a sample of HIV-positive African American mothers. Additional analyses explored whether women who had a history of a drug abuse or dependence diagnosis differed either on levels of the study variables or the model pathways. The results indicated that HIV-positive African American mothers who had higher levels of stressors perceived their stressors as a whole to be less controllable. Coping resources, available social support and perceived control, were positively associated with active coping and negatively associated with psychological distress. Avoidant coping was the most important predictor of psychological distress. Furthermore, the effect of avoidant coping on psychological distress was stronger for mothers with a history of drug diagnosis. The implications of these findings for targeting interventions are discussed. 相似文献
175.
Many students experience difficulty in acquiring basic writing skills and educators need to efficiently address those deficits
by implementing an intervention with a high likelihood for success. The current article demonstrates the utility of using
a brief experimental analysis (BEA) to identify a letter-formation intervention for a second-grade student with handwriting
difficulties. The results suggested that the BEA led to differentiated results for letter formation interventions, and the
identified intervention was implemented for 8 weeks with moderate effectiveness. Suggestions for future research are included. 相似文献
176.
Elizabeth Burns Coleman 《美学与艺术评论杂志》2001,59(4):385-402
177.
Domesticated quail (Coturnix japonica) received a discrete conditioned stimulus (CS) at one end of the experimental chamber paired with the opportunity to copulate with a female quail (the unconditioned stimulus) in a goal box located 112 cm away. Approach to the CS (sign tracking) and approach to the goal area (goal tracking) were measured. The duration of exposure to the experimental context (C) was varied in Experiment 1, and the duration of the conditioning trials (T) was varied in Experiment 2 for independent groups, creating C/T ratios of 1.0, 1.5, 4.5, 45, and 180. Contrary to previous reports of a direct relation between the C/T ratio and conditioned responding, in the present experiments, a shift in the topography and stimulus control of conditioned behavior occurred. Low C/T ratios (1.0-4.5) produced goal tracking controlled by contextual cues, whereas high C/T ratios (45 and 180) produced sign tracking controlled by the discrete CS. 相似文献
178.
Bruce D. Burns Regina Vollmeyer 《The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology》2002,55(1):241-261
Previous research has found that having a nonspecific goal (NSG) leads to better problem solving and transfer than having a specific goal (SG). To distinguish between the various explanations of this effect requires direct evidence showing how a NSG affects a participant's behaviour. Therefore we collected verbal protocols from participants learning to control a linear system consisting of 3 outputs by manipulating 3 inputs. This system was simpler than the one we had used previously, so in Exp. 1 we generalized our earlier goal specificity findings to this system. In Exp. 2 protocol analysis confirmed our prediction (based on dual-space theories of problem solving) that NSG participants focused on hypothesis testing whereas SG participants focused on the goal. However, this difference only emerged over time. We also replicated the goal specificity effect on performance and showed that giving participants a hypothesis to test improved performance. 相似文献
179.
Stacy Lee Burns 《Human Studies》2009,32(2):109-131
This paper examines the intersection of technical law and common sense reasoning in small claims arbitration, a distinctive
and increasingly prevalent kind of legal work. Following (Garfinkel, Ethnomethodology’s program: Working out Durkheim’s aphorism, 2002), the study explores the “reform of technical reason” and what a “just outcome” means by focusing on the arbitration of actual
small claims cases and how technical-legal and non-technical/informal resources are brought into alignment to produce dispute
resolution. The arbitrator elicits discussions that establish consensual and commonplace formulations of “the case,” formulations
that foreshadow its disposition as technical matters of law. The research demonstrates how formal structures of equity, evenhandedness,
and decisions without bias have their production in vivo, and how a just and fair course becomes a “just outcome.”
相似文献
Stacy Lee BurnsEmail: |
180.
Carol Fowler M.A. M.C. Susan R. Burns Ph.D. Janet E. Roehl 《Contemporary Family Therapy》1983,5(2):127-135
The use of group therapy is an essential component in the treatment program for victims of incest, their families, and offenders at the Center Against Sexual Assault in Phoenix, Arizona. Four types of groups are conducted on a continuous basis: (a) adults who were victims as children, (b) families of victims, (c) offenders, and (d) child victims. Group therapy is the predominant treatment modality for the 400 incestuous families seen at CASA. Details of each of the four groups are given in this article. 相似文献