全文获取类型
收费全文 | 785篇 |
免费 | 14篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
801篇 |
出版年
2019年 | 8篇 |
2018年 | 11篇 |
2017年 | 15篇 |
2016年 | 9篇 |
2015年 | 12篇 |
2014年 | 11篇 |
2013年 | 95篇 |
2012年 | 24篇 |
2011年 | 12篇 |
2010年 | 13篇 |
2009年 | 12篇 |
2008年 | 26篇 |
2007年 | 22篇 |
2006年 | 24篇 |
2005年 | 27篇 |
2004年 | 20篇 |
2003年 | 26篇 |
2002年 | 23篇 |
2001年 | 13篇 |
2000年 | 11篇 |
1999年 | 17篇 |
1998年 | 14篇 |
1997年 | 8篇 |
1995年 | 9篇 |
1994年 | 9篇 |
1993年 | 8篇 |
1991年 | 12篇 |
1990年 | 9篇 |
1989年 | 8篇 |
1988年 | 11篇 |
1987年 | 9篇 |
1985年 | 10篇 |
1984年 | 13篇 |
1983年 | 12篇 |
1982年 | 14篇 |
1981年 | 9篇 |
1980年 | 9篇 |
1979年 | 10篇 |
1978年 | 11篇 |
1977年 | 14篇 |
1976年 | 13篇 |
1975年 | 9篇 |
1974年 | 13篇 |
1973年 | 13篇 |
1971年 | 10篇 |
1970年 | 8篇 |
1969年 | 7篇 |
1968年 | 12篇 |
1967年 | 7篇 |
1966年 | 11篇 |
排序方式: 共有801条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
21.
Ralph J. Greenspan 《Science and engineering ethics》2012,18(3):447-452
Reductionist explanations in biology generally assume that biological mechanisms are highly deterministic and basically similar between individuals. A contrasting view has emerged recently that takes into account the degeneracy of biological processes??the ability to arrive at a given endpoint by a variety of available paths, even within the same individual. This perspective casts significant doubt on the prospects for the ability to predict behavior accurately based on brain imaging or genotyping, and on the ability of neuroscience to stipulate ethics. 相似文献
22.
Holloway RL 《The Behavioral and brain sciences》2012,35(4):226
Design features for language and stone toolmaking (not tool use) involve similar if not homologous cognitive processes. Both are arbitrary transformations of internal "intrinsic" symbolization, whereas non-human tool using is mostly an iconic transformation. The major discontinuity between humans and non-humans (chimpanzees) is language. The presence of stone tools made to standardized patterns suggests communicative and social control skills that involved language. 相似文献
23.
Psychometrika - The on-line version of this article has contained mistakes in affiliation part. 相似文献
24.
Neighborhood Stress and Life Satisfaction: Is there a Relationship for African American Adolescents?
Valois Robert F. Kerr Jelani C. Carey Michael P. Brown Larry K. Romer Daniel DiClemente Ralph J. Vanable Peter A. 《Applied research in quality of life》2020,15(1):273-296
Applied Research in Quality of Life - This study identified associations between perceived neighborhood stress and adolescents’ perceptions of life satisfaction. African American adolescents... 相似文献
25.
In both Pavlovian conditioning and human causal judgment, competition between cues is well known to occur when multiple cues are presented in compound and followed by an outcome. More questionable is the occurrence of competition between outcomes when a single cue is followed by multiple outcomes presented in compound. In the experiment reported here, we demonstrated blocking (a type of stimulus competition) between outcomes. When the cue predicted one outcome, its ability to predict a second outcome that was presented in compound with the first outcome was reduced. The procedure minimized the likelihood that the observed competition between outcomes arose from selective attention. The competition between outcomes that we observed is problematic for contemporary theories of learning. 相似文献
26.
Roughton RE 《Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association》2002,50(3):733-763
This presentation, written ten years after the American Psychoanalytic Association adopted a nondiscrimination policy with regard to sexual orientation, discusses the evolving relationship between psychoanalysis and homosexuality. The paper is in three sections: (1) the process of policy change and the overturning of injustice; (2) the excursion after Freud's death into analytic bias and extreme pathologizing of homosexuality, the struggle to overcome those distortions, and the lessons to be learned from this history; and (3) the search, in the coming decade, for new understanding of homosexuality and all aspects of sexuality. 相似文献
27.
Urushihara K Wheeler DS Pineño O Miller RR 《Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes》2005,31(2):184-198
Three conditioned taste aversion experiments with rats investigated superconditioning. In each experiment, alternate exposures of 2 flavor compounds with a common element (i.e., AB/AS) were administered to establish an inhibitory relationship between the 2 unique elements, B and S, and prior to testing, S was paired with lithium chloride (LiCl). In Experiment 1, pairings of a neutral cue (X) with S in compound with B after the AB/AS exposures resulted in superconditioning between X and S. Extinction of the common element (A) just before the S-LiCl pairing attenuated both the inhibitory relationship between B and S (Experiment 2) and superconditioning between X and S (Experiment 3). These observations suggest that superconditioning consists of enhanced performance rather than enhanced associative acquisition. 相似文献
28.
Denniston JC Blaisdell AP Miller RR 《Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes》2004,30(3):190-202
Two experiments with rats as subjects were conducted to investigate the associative structure of temporal control of conditioned inhibition through posttraining manipulation of the training excitor-unconditioned stimulus (US) temporal relationship. Experiment 1 found that following simultaneous Pavlovian inhibition training (i.e., A --> US/XA-no US) in which a conditioned stimulus (CS A) was established as a delay excitor, maximal inhibition was observed on a summation test when CS X was compounded with a delay transfer CS. Furthermore, posttraining shifts in the A-US temporal relationship from delay to trace resulted in maximal inhibition of a trace transfer CS. Experiment 2 found complementary results to Experiment 1 with an A-US posttraining shift from serial to simultaneous. These results suggest that temporal control of inhibition is mediated by the training excitor-US temporal relationship. 相似文献
29.
Dissociable neural systems for recognizing emotions 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
This study tested the hypothesis that the recognition of emotions would draw upon anatomically separable brain regions, depending on whether the stimuli were static or explicitly conveyed information regarding actions. We investigated the hypothesis in a rare subject with extensive bilateral brain lesions, patient B., by administering tasks that assessed recognition and naming of emotions from visual and verbal stimuli, some of which depicted actions and some of which did not. B. could not recognize any primary emotion other than happiness, when emotions were shown as static images or given as single verbal labels. By contrast, with the notable exception of disgust, he correctly recognized primary emotions from dynamic displays of facial expressions as well as from stories that described actions. Our findings are consistent with the idea that information about actions is processed in occipitoparietal and dorsal frontal cortices, all of which are intact in B.'s brain. Such information subsequently would be linked to knowledge about emotions that depends on structures mapping somatic states, many of which are also intact in B.'s brain. However, one of these somatosensory structures, the insula, is bilaterally damaged, perhaps accounting for B.'s uniformly impaired recognition of disgust (from both static and action stimuli). Other structures that are damaged in B.'s brain, including bilateral inferior and anterior temporal lobe and medial frontal cortices, appear to be critical for linking perception of static stimuli to recognition of emotions. Thus the retrieval of knowledge regarding emotions draws upon widely distributed and partly distinct sets of neural structures, depending on the attributes of the stimulus. 相似文献
30.
Once people know the outcome of an event, they tend to overestimate what could have been anticipated in foresight. Although typically considered to be a robust phenomenon, this hindsight bias is subject to moderating circumstances. In their meta-analysis, Christensen-Szalanski and Willham (1991) observed that the more experience people have with the task under consideration, the smaller is the resulting hindsight bias. This observation is one benchmark against which the explanatory power of process models of hindsight bias can be measured. Therefore, we used it to put the recently proposed RAFT model (Hoffrage, Hertwig, & Gigerenzer, 2000) to another test. Our findings were consistent with the "expertise effect." Specifically, we observed-using computer simulations of the RAFT model-that the more comprehensive people's knowledge is in foresight, the smaller is their hindsight bias. In addition, we made two counterintuitive observations: First, the relation between foresight knowledge and hindsight bias appears to be independent of how knowledge is processed. Second, even if foresight knowledge is false, it can reduce hindsight bias. We conclude with a discussion of the functional value of hindsight bias. 相似文献