全文获取类型
收费全文 | 502篇 |
免费 | 5篇 |
专业分类
507篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 3篇 |
2023年 | 5篇 |
2022年 | 6篇 |
2021年 | 11篇 |
2020年 | 12篇 |
2019年 | 9篇 |
2018年 | 24篇 |
2017年 | 11篇 |
2016年 | 21篇 |
2015年 | 5篇 |
2014年 | 12篇 |
2013年 | 77篇 |
2012年 | 21篇 |
2011年 | 32篇 |
2010年 | 35篇 |
2009年 | 24篇 |
2008年 | 21篇 |
2007年 | 21篇 |
2006年 | 21篇 |
2005年 | 11篇 |
2004年 | 24篇 |
2003年 | 11篇 |
2002年 | 12篇 |
2001年 | 4篇 |
2000年 | 3篇 |
1999年 | 5篇 |
1998年 | 3篇 |
1997年 | 2篇 |
1996年 | 6篇 |
1995年 | 2篇 |
1994年 | 2篇 |
1992年 | 4篇 |
1991年 | 4篇 |
1990年 | 3篇 |
1989年 | 2篇 |
1988年 | 4篇 |
1986年 | 1篇 |
1985年 | 2篇 |
1984年 | 4篇 |
1983年 | 5篇 |
1982年 | 2篇 |
1981年 | 2篇 |
1980年 | 3篇 |
1978年 | 2篇 |
1976年 | 4篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
1974年 | 2篇 |
1971年 | 1篇 |
1970年 | 1篇 |
1969年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有507条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
101.
In two experiments, study-list composition was manipulated and its impact was observed on metacognitive judgements associated with recognition hits (Hs) and false alarms (FAs). Both studies involved recognition of high-frequency (HF) and low-frequency (LF) words, and focused on the FA portion of the word frequency effect. Our hypothesis was that participants can actively reject items that are distinctive from the study list, and that this effect may be strong enough to overcome the FA word frequency effect. Experiment 1 manipulated list composition with a conjunctive rule and Experiment 2 varied it by having participants study either HF or LF items prior to a test consisting of words of both frequencies. In each study H rate, FA rate, and metacognitive attributions underlying recognition decisions were investigated. In both studies, participants reported rejecting test items predominantly through a process of active rejection, which was more often reported for LF items. This effect was strong enough to reverse the FA portion of the word frequency effect in Experiment 2, but not Experiment 1. The results are discussed in terms of metacognitive rejection mechanisms in recognition memory. 相似文献
102.
103.
Bruno Verbeek 《Ethical Theory and Moral Practice》2010,13(5):541-559
In this essay, I review some results that suggest that rational choice theory has interesting things to say about the virtues.
In particular, I argue that rational choice theory can show, first, the role of certain virtues in a game-theoretic analysis
of norms. Secondly, that it is useful in the characterization of these virtues. Finally, I discuss how rational choice theory
can be brought to bear upon the justification of these virtues by showing how they contribute to a flourishing life. I do
this by discussing one particular example of a norm - the requirement that agents to honor their promises of mutual assistance
- and one particular virtue, trustworthiness. 相似文献
104.
Dana Anaby Tal Jarus Catherine L. Backman Bruno D. Zumbo 《Applied research in quality of life》2010,5(2):81-104
One of the key factors for promoting well-being lies in balancing one’s daily life occupations and the nature of these occupations.
Occupation refers to a group of everyday life goal-directed activities which may be associated to any life domain, not necessarily
work, such as leisure, education, or self-care. Yet it is not clear what constitutes occupational imbalance, and its association
to other factors has not been examined systematically. This study proposed and tested a theoretical model for specifying the
structural relationships between occupational imbalance, occupational characteristics, personality and well-being. 288 working
adults completed the following questionnaires: Inter-goal Relations Questionnaire (occupational imbalance), Personal Projects
Analysis (occupational characteristics), the Big Five Inventory (personality traits) and the Satisfaction with Life Scale
(well-being). Twenty-five models were tested using structural equation modeling. All the models fit the data well. Occupational
characteristics, with the exception of stress, served as significant mediators between personality and well-being, yet occupational
imbalance did not play a significant role across all models. Our findings provide substantial support for the importance of
occupation to well-being, however occupational imbalance is still an elusive concept that requires further investigation theoretically
and empirically. 相似文献
105.
The present study was designed to examine how the correlations between cognitive abilities evolve during adulthood. Data from 1104 participants on the French version of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Third Edition were analyzed. The entire sample was divided into four age groups (16–24 years; 25–44 years; 45–69 years and 70–89 years), which were themselves split into two IQ-levels using the mean standard score on Vocabulary and Block Design. For every age group, the mean correlation between subtest scores of low-IQ participants was higher than that of high-IQ participants. There was also no interaction between age and IQ for the strength of subtest relationships. Indeed, the effect sizes of correlation differences between low- and high-IQ participants appeared to be relatively constant across age. A general developmental schema of how the strength of correlations between cognitive abilities of low- and high-IQ individuals evolves during the entire life span is sketched in the discussion. 相似文献
106.
Philosophical Studies - Normative powers like promising allow agents to effect changes to their reasons, permissions and rights by the means of communicative actions whose function is to effect... 相似文献
107.
Bruno M. Shah OP 《新多明我会修道士》2009,90(1025):38-49
Ian McEwan is arguably the best living British novelist. His most successful novel, Atonement , was recently made into an internationally successful film. And indeed, through analysis of his novels, it is clear that Ian McEwan believes literature—precisely as fictive—might very well bear the task of atonement for postmodernity. His novels, though, are patently hopeless, (even as they are truly well-written). Because McEwan doesn't accept or see the causes of sin as such—formally understood as rebellion against the Creator—his diagnostic aesthetic of our postmodern malaise is incomplete and ineffectual. The literary or fictive atonement that he would achieve through his novels does not satisfy. This article aims to lay bare the philosophico-literary characteristics of Ian McEwan's later novels. The ultimate goal of this critical reading, though—tending toward an "evangelical lection"—is to transfigure McEwan's imaginative and creative virtuosity for otherwise disappointed Christian readers, precisely by envisioning his novels in the dark light of their redemptive deficit. Thus, the literary or fictive atonement that Ian McEwan's atheism cannot achieve might be saved apropos the Judeo-Christian revelation of divine atonement. 相似文献
108.
Luisa Sartori Cristina Becchio Bruno G. Bara Umberto Castiello 《Consciousness and cognition》2009,18(3):766-772
The aim of the present study was to investigate the effects of communicative intention on action. In Experiment 1 participants were requested to reach towards an object, grasp it, and either simply lift it (individual condition) or lift it with the intent to communicate a meaning to a partner (communicative condition). Movement kinematics were recorded using a three-dimensional motion analysis system. The results indicate that kinematics was sensitive to communicative intention. Although the to-be-grasped object remained the same, movements performed for the ‘communicative’ condition were characterized by a kinematic pattern which differed from those obtained for the ‘individual’ condition. These findings were confirmed in a subsequent experiment in which the communicative condition was compared to a control condition, in which the communicative exchange was prevented. Results are discussed in terms of cognitive pragmatics and current knowledge on how social behavior shapes action kinematics. 相似文献
109.
Peter E. Keller Bruno H. Repp 《The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology》2004,57(6):1085-1101
This study addresses the demands of alternating bimanual syncopation, a coordination mode in which the two hands move in alternation while tapping in antiphase with a metronomic tone sequence. Musically trained participants were required to engage in alternating bimanual syncopation and five other coordination modes: unimanual syncopation where taps are made (with the left or right hand) after every tone; unimanual syncopation where taps are made after every other tone; bimanual synchronization with alternating hands; unimanual synchronized tapping with every tone; and unimanual tapping with every other tone. Variability in tap timing was greatest overall for alternating bimanual syncopation, indicating that it is the most difficult. This appears to be due to instability arising from the simultaneous presence of two levels of antiphase coordination (one between the pacing sequence and the hands, the other between the two hands) rather than factors relating to movement frequency or dexterity limits of the nonpreferred hand. 相似文献
110.
Bruno H. Repp 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1995,57(8):1217-1232
Two experiments demonstrate positional variation in the relative detectability of, respectively, local temporal and dynamic perturbations in an isochronous and isodynamic sequence of melody tones, played on a computer-controlled piano. This variation may reflect listeners’ expectations of expressive performance microstructure (thetop-down hypothesis), or it may be due to psychoacoustic (pitch-related) stimulus factors (thebottom-up hypothesis). Percent correct scores for increments in tone duration correlated significantly with the average timing profile of pianists’ expressive performances of the music, as predicted specifically by the top-down hypothesis. For intensity increments, the analogous perception-performance correlation was weak and the bottom-up factors of relative pitch height and/or direction of pitch change accounted for some of the perceptual variation. Subjects’ musical training increased overall detection accuracy but did not affect the positional variation in accuracy scores in either experiment. These results are consistent with the top-down hypothesis for timing, but they favor the bottom-up hypothesis for dynamics. The perception-performance correlation for timing may also be viewed as being due to complex stimulus properties such as tonal motion and tension/relaxation that influence performers and listeners in similar ways. 相似文献