全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1741篇 |
免费 | 41篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
1784篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 13篇 |
2019年 | 22篇 |
2018年 | 27篇 |
2017年 | 22篇 |
2016年 | 34篇 |
2015年 | 19篇 |
2014年 | 22篇 |
2013年 | 163篇 |
2012年 | 65篇 |
2011年 | 63篇 |
2010年 | 39篇 |
2009年 | 26篇 |
2008年 | 41篇 |
2007年 | 64篇 |
2006年 | 51篇 |
2005年 | 51篇 |
2004年 | 68篇 |
2003年 | 54篇 |
2002年 | 73篇 |
2001年 | 42篇 |
2000年 | 51篇 |
1999年 | 30篇 |
1998年 | 58篇 |
1997年 | 37篇 |
1996年 | 19篇 |
1995年 | 28篇 |
1994年 | 25篇 |
1993年 | 26篇 |
1992年 | 40篇 |
1991年 | 32篇 |
1990年 | 20篇 |
1989年 | 24篇 |
1988年 | 23篇 |
1987年 | 20篇 |
1986年 | 25篇 |
1985年 | 26篇 |
1984年 | 19篇 |
1983年 | 16篇 |
1982年 | 31篇 |
1981年 | 27篇 |
1980年 | 25篇 |
1979年 | 16篇 |
1978年 | 22篇 |
1977年 | 14篇 |
1976年 | 13篇 |
1975年 | 19篇 |
1974年 | 27篇 |
1972年 | 13篇 |
1969年 | 13篇 |
1968年 | 11篇 |
排序方式: 共有1784条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
71.
John G. Arena Edward B. Blanchard Frank Andrasik Bruce C. Dudek 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》1982,4(1):55-69
A brief Headache Symptom Questionnaire was administered to 129 chronic headache Sufferers. The questionnaire accurately classified 68.42% of headache subjects in their proper diagnostic category, comparable to, but statistically less accurate than, the 86.4% agreement between expert headache diagnosticians using clinical interviews. Results of a factor analysis of the Headache Symptom Questionnaire lend support for their being two commonly accepted global headache categories—vascular/migraine and muscle contraction—and one headache dimension concerned with duration of headache pain. Combined migraine-muscle contraction headache was found to be related more to migraine than to muscle contraction headache, and cluster headaches emerged as a separate clinical entity, not loading positively on any factor and loading negatively on all three.This research was supported in part by Grant NS-15235 from the National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke. 相似文献
72.
Effects of aversive CSs upon dogs' instrumental avoidance responding were assessed as a function of the Pavlovian conditioning parameters using off-baseline (Experiment 1) and on-baseline (Experiment 2) conditioning procedures. In each experiment, three UCS intensitites (2, 6, and 10 mA) were crossed factorially with two CS-UCS intervals (10 and 90 sec) yielding six groups. After 4 days of conditioning, the CS-produced effects on avoidance were determined primarily by an interaction between Pavlovian procedure (off- versus on-baseline) and UCS intensity. With off-baseline conditioning, CS-produced facilitation of avoidance was an increasing function of the UCS intensity. However, with on-baseline conditioning, only the CS paired with weak UCS facilitated avoidance, and the CS paired with the strong UCS suppressed responding. This reversing parametric function poses problems for two-process motivational theories of avoidance. 相似文献
73.
A theory of intelligence is couched in stimulus-response terms, bridging the gap between S-R and cognitive psychology. The chief theoretical concepts are span ability (a capacity notion), response string (sequentially cued responses), and complexity of stimulus control (task complexity). Span is equated with the ability to respond appropriately when several cues are conjunctively relevant for correct performance (complex stimulus control). So defined, span is consistently and broadly related to many aspects of intelligent behavior. 相似文献
74.
The study was designed to explore two areas: (1) the concurrent validity of Holland's theory for employed college degreed women using two different operational definitions (Vocational Preference Inventory and the Self-Directed Search) of vocational orientation; and (2) the relationships among same named scales across the VPI and the SDS. Concurrent validity was studied by administering the VPI and the SDS to 179 women workers in occupational environments consistent with Holland's six vocational environments. In general, the findings indicate that the VPI and SDS scales tend to effectively discriminate among the occupational groups consistent with Holland's theoretical framework. In addition, the correlation coefficients for same named scales for the two inventories were all found to be significant. In sum, the results of the study lend some support to the concurrent validity of Holland's theory for employed college degreed women. 相似文献
75.
Previous research had indicated that there were differences in the kinds of stimulus information processed by concentrated and distributed attention in peripheral vision. Concentrated attention was necessary for perceiving line arrangement differences, while line slant differences could be detected with distributed attention. However, experiments with foveal presentation showed no facilitation by concentrating attention. Experiment I replicated these results. Experiment II showed that distributed attention did degrade foveal line arrangement discrimination if the attention system was overloaded by increasing the number of elements in the array. Experiment III demonstrated that high element density was not a sufficient condition for these effects to occur. Line arrangement discrimination was reduced as letter number increased even when maximum density was held constant. In Experim~ent IV, it was shown that concentrating attention facilitated line arrangement discrimination relative to line slant discrimination in foveal vision when element number and density were held constant. The results are discussed in terms of several models postulating a difference between spread-out and concentrated attention systems. 相似文献
76.
77.
Periods of 24 to 48 hours of food deprivation reliably induced pup-killing in 30–50% of non-killer male mice. The behavior was prevented by previous experience with young and did not perseverate to non-deprived states. Castrated males and intact females also exhibited pup-killing following food deprivation, suggesting that the behavior is neither sexdependent nor related to the presence of testosterone. The findings are discussed in terms of their relationship to predatory behaviors and population dynamics. 相似文献
78.
79.
Bruce AE Cole DA Dallaire DH Jacquez FM Pineda AQ LaGrange B 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》2006,34(3):310-322
In a sample of 299 children (grades 2, 4, and 6), we examined parenting and negative life events as predictors of depressive cognitions, specifically low self-perceived competence, depressive cognitive schemas, and depressogenic attributional style. We also examined developmental trends in these relations. Children completed measures of parenting, negative life events, and depressive cognitions. Parents also completed measures of parenting and negative life events. Consistent with our hypotheses, negative parenting and negative life events corresponded with higher levels of depressive cognitions, whereas positive parenting corresponded with lower levels of depressive cognitions. The relations between negative parenting and negative automatic thoughts were stronger for older children. Theoretical and practical implications are discussed. 相似文献
80.