全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2138篇 |
免费 | 27篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
2167篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 15篇 |
2020年 | 19篇 |
2019年 | 34篇 |
2018年 | 38篇 |
2017年 | 39篇 |
2016年 | 45篇 |
2015年 | 27篇 |
2014年 | 38篇 |
2013年 | 193篇 |
2012年 | 77篇 |
2011年 | 89篇 |
2010年 | 63篇 |
2009年 | 43篇 |
2008年 | 49篇 |
2007年 | 81篇 |
2006年 | 64篇 |
2005年 | 64篇 |
2004年 | 78篇 |
2003年 | 59篇 |
2002年 | 83篇 |
2001年 | 50篇 |
2000年 | 57篇 |
1999年 | 35篇 |
1998年 | 60篇 |
1997年 | 38篇 |
1996年 | 21篇 |
1995年 | 30篇 |
1994年 | 30篇 |
1993年 | 27篇 |
1992年 | 42篇 |
1991年 | 36篇 |
1990年 | 24篇 |
1989年 | 29篇 |
1988年 | 30篇 |
1987年 | 23篇 |
1986年 | 27篇 |
1985年 | 30篇 |
1984年 | 22篇 |
1983年 | 18篇 |
1982年 | 31篇 |
1981年 | 29篇 |
1980年 | 26篇 |
1979年 | 17篇 |
1978年 | 26篇 |
1977年 | 17篇 |
1976年 | 16篇 |
1975年 | 22篇 |
1974年 | 29篇 |
1972年 | 14篇 |
1969年 | 17篇 |
排序方式: 共有2167条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
971.
In four experiments, the impact of concreteness of language on judgments of truth was examined. In Experiments 1 and 2, it was found that statements of the very same content were judged as more probably true when they were written in concrete language than when they were written in abstract language. Findings of Experiment 2 also showed that this linguistic concreteness effect on judgments of truth could most likely be attributed to greater perceived vividness of concrete compared to abstract statements. Two further experiments demonstrated an additional fit effect: The truth advantage of concrete statements occurred especially when participants were primed with a concrete (vs. abstract) mind-set (Experiment 3) or when the statements were presented in a spatially proximal (vs. distant) location (Experiment 4). Implications for communication strategies are discussed. 相似文献
972.
Kate A. Longstaffe Bruce M. Hood Iain D. Gilchrist 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》2014,76(1):49-63
During search, executive function enables individuals to direct attention to potential targets, remember locations visited, and inhibit distracting information. In the present study, we investigated these executive processes in large-scale search. In our tasks, participants searched a room containing an array of illuminated locations embedded in the floor. The participants’ task was to press the switches at the illuminated locations on the floor so as to locate a target that changed color when pressed. The perceptual salience of the search locations was manipulated by having some locations flashing and some static. Participants were more likely to search at flashing locations, even when they were explicitly informed that the target was equally likely to be at any location. In large-scale search, attention was captured by the perceptual salience of the flashing lights, leading to a bias to explore these targets. Despite this failure of inhibition, participants were able to restrict returns to previously visited locations, a measure of spatial memory performance. Participants were more able to inhibit exploration to flashing locations when they were not required to remember which locations had previously been visited. A concurrent digit-span memory task further disrupted inhibition during search, as did a concurrent auditory attention task. These experiments extend a load theory of attention to large-scale search, which relies on egocentric representations of space. High cognitive load on working memory leads to increased distractor interference, providing evidence for distinct roles for the executive subprocesses of memory and inhibition during large-scale search. 相似文献
973.
974.
Kronlund A Whittlesea BW 《Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition》2006,32(5):1174-1184
A surprising validation of expectation experienced during a recognition test induces the perception of discrepancy and a feeling of familiarity. The authors investigated whether that perception also affects memory performance when it is experienced in the original encounter with a stimulus. Target words were presented in a study phase, half in a context thought to induce the perception of discrepancy. In a subsequent recognition test, that earlier experience increased the accuracy of subjects' discrimination. However, when the subsequent task required a once-versus-twice judgment, that experience caused an illusion of reoccurrence for words presented once. The authors concluded that a perception of discrepancy in an initial encounter may be a valuable aid to later recognition but can also cause systematic memory errors under some circumstances. 相似文献
975.
White S Milne E Rosen S Hansen P Swettenham J Frith U Ramus F 《Developmental science》2006,9(3):237-55; discussion 265-9
This study attempts to investigate the role of sensorimotor impairments in the reading disability that characterizes dyslexia. Twenty-three children with dyslexia were compared to 22 control children, matched for age and non-verbal intelligence, on tasks assessing literacy as well as phonological, visual, auditory and motor abilities. The dyslexic group as a whole were significantly impaired on phonological, but not sensorimotor, tasks. Analysis of individual data suggests that the most common impairments were on phonological and visual stress tasks and the vast majority of dyslexics had one of these two impairments. Furthermore, phonological skill was able to account for variation in literacy skill, to the exclusion of all sensorimotor factors, while neither auditory nor motor skill predicted any variance in phonological skill. Visual stress seems to account for a small proportion of dyslexics, independently of the commonly reported phonological deficit. However, there is little evidence for a causal role of auditory, motor or other visual impairments. 相似文献
976.
The authors propose a taxonomy of social identities, suggesting that three different classifications of identities can be distinguished. These comprise those that are innate and visible, such as race or gender; those that are innate, but invisible, such as sexual orientation; and those that are acquired or achieved, such as marital status or political affiliation. The authors argue that each of these categories has different implications for the revelation or disclosure of aspects of the therapist's identity, as well as for transference-countertransference dynamics. These points are illustrated with brief clinical examples. 相似文献
977.
Comparison of two self-report instruments for assessing binge eating in bariatric surgery candidates
Elder KA Grilo CM Masheb RM Rothschild BS Burke-Martindale CH Brody ML 《Behaviour research and therapy》2006,44(4):545-560
This study compared two self-report methods for assessing binge eating in severely obese bariatric surgery candidates. Participants were 249 gastric bypass candidates who completed the Questionnaire on Eating and Weight Patterns-Revised (QEWP-R) and the Eating Disorder Examination-Questionnaire (EDE-Q) prior to surgery. Participants were classified by binge eating status (i.e., no or recurrent binge eating) with each of the measures. The degree of agreement was examined, as well as the relationship between binge eating and measures of convergent validity. The two measures identified a similar number of patients with recurrent binge eating (i.e., at least 1 binge/week); however, overlap was modest (kappa=.26). Agreement on twice weekly binge eating was poor (kappa=.05). The QEWP-R and EDE-Q both identified clinically meaningful groups of binge eaters. The EDE-Q appeared to differentiate between non/infrequent bingers and recurrent bingers better than the QEWP-R, based on measures of convergent validity. In addition, the EDE-Q demonstrated an advantage because it identified binge eaters with elevated weight and shape overconcern. Using the self-report measures concurrently did not improve identification of binge eating in this study. More work is needed to determine the construct validity and clinical utility of these measures with gastric bypass patients. 相似文献
978.
This study tested the segmentation hypothesis of dyslexia by measuring implicit phonological representations in reading-disabled 11- to 13-year-olds. Implicit measures included lexical gating, priming, and syllable similarity tasks designed to reduce metalinguistic demands. Children with dyslexia performed consistently worse than CA and RA controls when more segmental representations were required across all three tasks. Implicit phonological representations were correlated with measures of speech perception, phoneme awareness, and phonological short-term memory, but not rapid automatized naming, and accounted for unique variance in predicting reading ability. Results provide strong support for less mature implicit phonological representations in children with dyslexia. 相似文献
979.
How are we to understand the difficult expression raisedfor our justification (Romans 4:25b)? The clue lies intaking seriously the two + accusatives in Romans 4:23. Therethey express a parallel between the promise of righteousnessfor him (Abraham) and the same kind of promise,still valid for us. This same emphasis shouldbe taken into 4:25, for us ... for us. Such areading is not only backed up by the Isaiah 53 echo and thecontents of Romans 5, it also serves to explain the meaningof Romans 4:25b. Abraham was given an opportunity for righteousness-producingfaith through a tension between under-realizedreality and God's faithfulness (vv.1822). Throughthe resurrection of Jesus Christ, the same opportunity is nowafforded to us. 相似文献
980.
Hoffman PD Fruzzetti AE Buteau E Neiditch ER Penney D Bruce ML Hellman F Struening E 《Family process》2005,44(2):217-225
This study assessed changes in family members who participated in Family Connections, a 12-week manualized education program for relatives of persons with borderline personality disorder (BPD). Family Connections, led by trained family members, is based on the strategies of standard Dialectical Behavior Therapy (DBT) and DBT for families. The program provides (a) current information and research on BPD, (b) coping skills, (c) family skills, and (d) opportunities to build a support network for family members. Forty-four participants representing 34 families completed the pre-, post-, and 6-month postbaseline self-report questionnaires. Analyses employing hierarchical linear modeling strategies showed significant reductions in grief and burden, and a significant increase in mastery from pre- to post-group assessment. Changes were maintained at 6 months post baseline. 相似文献