首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   79958篇
  免费   3181篇
  国内免费   26篇
  2019年   935篇
  2018年   1301篇
  2017年   1340篇
  2016年   1415篇
  2015年   1033篇
  2014年   1242篇
  2013年   5949篇
  2012年   2278篇
  2011年   2473篇
  2010年   1552篇
  2009年   1533篇
  2008年   2214篇
  2007年   2181篇
  2006年   2027篇
  2005年   1772篇
  2004年   1678篇
  2003年   1593篇
  2002年   1709篇
  2001年   2417篇
  2000年   2384篇
  1999年   1850篇
  1998年   917篇
  1997年   856篇
  1996年   858篇
  1992年   1598篇
  1991年   1471篇
  1990年   1463篇
  1989年   1346篇
  1988年   1338篇
  1987年   1264篇
  1986年   1379篇
  1985年   1388篇
  1984年   1180篇
  1983年   1069篇
  1982年   768篇
  1981年   777篇
  1979年   1263篇
  1978年   926篇
  1977年   767篇
  1976年   776篇
  1975年   1042篇
  1974年   1165篇
  1973年   1275篇
  1972年   1038篇
  1971年   1017篇
  1970年   911篇
  1969年   957篇
  1968年   1205篇
  1967年   1079篇
  1966年   986篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
81.
In five experiments, we examined 3‐ to 6‐year‐olds’ understanding that they could gain knowledge indirectly from someone who had seen something they had not. Consistent with previous research, children judged that an informant, who had seen inside a box, knew its contents. Similarly, when an informant marked a picture to indicate her suggestion as to the content of the box, 3‐ to 4‐year‐olds trusted this more frequently when the informant had seen inside the box than when she had not. Going beyond previous research, 3‐ to 4‐year‐olds were also sensitive to informants’ relevant experience when they had to look over a barrier to see the marked picture, or ask for the barrier to be raised. Yet when children had to elicit the informant's suggestion, rather than just consult a suggestion already present, even 4‐ to 5‐year‐olds were no more likely to do so when the informant had seen the box's content than when she had not, and no more likely to trust the well‐informed suggestion than the uninformed one. We conclude that young children who can ask questions may not yet fully understand the process by which they can gain accurate information from someone who has the experience they lack.  相似文献   
82.
This study examines the relationship of life stress, daily hassles, and perceived self-efficacy to adjustment in a community sample of 32 men and 32 women between ages 65 and 75. In a structured interview, negative life change events, daily hassles, self-efficacy, depression, psychosomatic symptoms, and negative well being were assessed. Both negative life events and daily hassles were related to psychological distress and physical symptoms for men, and hassles were associated with psychological distress and physical symptoms for women. An inverse relationship between self-efficacy and maladjustment was also found. Hassles showed the most powerful relationship to distress.  相似文献   
83.
Book reviews     
TOATES, F. Animal Behaviour: A Systems Approach. Chichester: John Wiley and Sons. 1980. Pp. 299. Hardback £17. Paper back £6.40 ISBN 0 471 27724.

DICKINSON, A. Contemporary Animal Learning Theory. Cambridge: University Press. 1980. Pp. 177. Hardback £12.50. Paperback £3.95. ISBN 0 521 23469 7.  相似文献   
84.
85.
86.
87.
88.
89.
90.
How do speakers design what they say in order to communicate effectively with groups of addressees who vary in their background knowledge of the topic at hand? Prior findings indicate that when a speaker addresses a pair of listeners with discrepant knowledge, that speakers Aim Low, designing their utterances for the least knowledgeable of the two addressees. Here, we test the hypothesis that speakers will depart from an Aim Low approach in order to efficiently communicate with larger groups of interacting partners. Further, we ask whether the cognitive demands of tracking multiple conversational partners' perspectives places limitations on successful audience design. We find that speakers can successfully track information about what up to four of their partners do and do not know in conversation. When addressing groups of 3–4 addressees at once, speakers design language based on the combined knowledge of the group. These findings point to an audience design process that simultaneously represents the perspectives of multiple other individuals and combines these representations in order to design utterances that strike a balance between the different needs of the individuals within the group.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号