首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   39244篇
  免费   1538篇
  国内免费   15篇
  2020年   462篇
  2019年   569篇
  2018年   777篇
  2017年   846篇
  2016年   851篇
  2015年   598篇
  2014年   722篇
  2013年   3121篇
  2012年   1314篇
  2011年   1329篇
  2010年   850篇
  2009年   814篇
  2008年   1203篇
  2007年   1205篇
  2006年   1087篇
  2005年   950篇
  2004年   901篇
  2003年   830篇
  2002年   864篇
  2001年   1191篇
  2000年   1160篇
  1999年   908篇
  1998年   402篇
  1997年   391篇
  1996年   366篇
  1995年   362篇
  1993年   362篇
  1992年   764篇
  1991年   667篇
  1990年   725篇
  1989年   623篇
  1988年   662篇
  1987年   616篇
  1986年   612篇
  1985年   559篇
  1984年   514篇
  1983年   495篇
  1979年   591篇
  1978年   405篇
  1977年   353篇
  1975年   464篇
  1974年   512篇
  1973年   537篇
  1972年   410篇
  1971年   404篇
  1970年   361篇
  1969年   416篇
  1968年   491篇
  1967年   442篇
  1966年   366篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
41.
Previous research suggests that children gradually understand the mitigating effects of apology on damage to a transgressor's reputation. However, little is known about young children's insights into the central emotional implications of apology. In two studies, children ages 4–9 heard stories about moral transgressions in which the wrongdoers either did or did not apologize. In Study 1, children in the no‐apology condition showed the classic pattern of ‘happy victimizer’ attributions by expecting the wrongdoer to feel good about gains won via transgression. By contrast, in the apology condition, children attributed negative feelings to the transgressor and improved feelings to the victim. In Study 2, these effects were found even when the explicit emotion marker ‘sorry’ was removed from the apology exchange. Thus, young children understand some important emotional functions of apology.  相似文献   
42.
The current study hypothesized that (1) hope would negatively predict burdensomeness, thwarted belongingness, and acquired capability to enact lethal injury; (2) hope would negatively predict suicidal ideation; and (3) the interpersonal suicide risk factors would predict suicidal ideation. Results indicated that hope negatively predicted burdensomeness and thwarted belongingness, but positively predicted acquired capability to enact suicide. Contrary to our second hypothesis, hope did not predict suicidal ideation, but interpersonal risk factors for suicide predicted suicidal ideation. Results are discussed in terms of implications for hope theory and Joiner's (2005) interpersonal risk factors for suicide, and for clinical practice.  相似文献   
43.
44.
How do speakers design what they say in order to communicate effectively with groups of addressees who vary in their background knowledge of the topic at hand? Prior findings indicate that when a speaker addresses a pair of listeners with discrepant knowledge, that speakers Aim Low, designing their utterances for the least knowledgeable of the two addressees. Here, we test the hypothesis that speakers will depart from an Aim Low approach in order to efficiently communicate with larger groups of interacting partners. Further, we ask whether the cognitive demands of tracking multiple conversational partners' perspectives places limitations on successful audience design. We find that speakers can successfully track information about what up to four of their partners do and do not know in conversation. When addressing groups of 3–4 addressees at once, speakers design language based on the combined knowledge of the group. These findings point to an audience design process that simultaneously represents the perspectives of multiple other individuals and combines these representations in order to design utterances that strike a balance between the different needs of the individuals within the group.  相似文献   
45.
46.
47.
48.
49.
50.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号