全文获取类型
收费全文 | 453篇 |
免费 | 20篇 |
专业分类
473篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 7篇 |
2021年 | 5篇 |
2020年 | 6篇 |
2019年 | 5篇 |
2018年 | 7篇 |
2017年 | 6篇 |
2016年 | 15篇 |
2015年 | 12篇 |
2014年 | 12篇 |
2013年 | 44篇 |
2012年 | 17篇 |
2011年 | 20篇 |
2010年 | 10篇 |
2009年 | 14篇 |
2008年 | 13篇 |
2007年 | 16篇 |
2006年 | 26篇 |
2005年 | 13篇 |
2004年 | 6篇 |
2003年 | 13篇 |
2002年 | 8篇 |
2001年 | 8篇 |
2000年 | 13篇 |
1999年 | 8篇 |
1998年 | 11篇 |
1997年 | 4篇 |
1996年 | 7篇 |
1995年 | 6篇 |
1994年 | 5篇 |
1992年 | 5篇 |
1991年 | 5篇 |
1990年 | 7篇 |
1989年 | 7篇 |
1988年 | 5篇 |
1987年 | 7篇 |
1986年 | 5篇 |
1985年 | 10篇 |
1984年 | 13篇 |
1982年 | 4篇 |
1981年 | 3篇 |
1979年 | 5篇 |
1978年 | 4篇 |
1977年 | 6篇 |
1975年 | 4篇 |
1972年 | 6篇 |
1971年 | 5篇 |
1969年 | 3篇 |
1968年 | 4篇 |
1967年 | 3篇 |
1965年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有473条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
151.
Peter Harrison 《Zygon》2016,51(3):742-757
I am grateful to the four reviewers of The Territories of Science and Religion for their careful and insightful readings of the book, and their kind words about it. They all got the central arguments pretty much right, and thus any critical comments are not the result of fundamental misunderstandings. While there are some common themes in the assessments, each reviewer, happily, has offered a distinct perspective on the book. For this reason I will deal with their comments in turn, but with a focus throughout on a generally expressed concern about the broader implications of the book's historical analysis, and what positive or concrete proposals might follow from it. 相似文献
152.
Stensen Kenneth Lydersen Stian Stenseng Frode Wallander Jan Lance Drugli May Britt 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》2021,43(3):646-656
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment - Identification attempts in populations with a low prevalence of problems usually result in a considerable number of false positives. Thus, the... 相似文献
153.
154.
Chafouleas SM Kilgus SP Riley-Tillman TC Jaffery R Harrison S 《Journal of School Psychology》2012,50(3):317-334
This study examined the impact of various components of rater training on the accuracy of rating behavior using Direct Behavior Rating-Single Item Scales (DBR-SIS). Specifically, the addition of frame-of-reference and rater error training components to a standard package involving an overview and then modeling, practice, and feedback was investigated. In addition, amount of exposure to the direct training component (i.e., number of practice and feedback opportunities) was evaluated, and the rates at which behavior was displayed were carefully manipulated to control for and evaluate training impact by target and rate of behavior. The sample consisted of undergraduate students assigned to one of 6 possible conditions. Overall findings suggested that completion of a training package did result in enhanced accuracy when using DBR-SIS to rate academic engagement and disruption. However, results also supported that the most comprehensive package of DBR training may not always result in greater improvements over a standard package involving direct training. In general, a more intensive training package appeared beneficial at improving ratings for targets that had previously been difficult to rate accurately (e.g., medium rate disruptive behavior). Limitations and implications for future research are discussed. 相似文献
155.
In two experiments, we examined the role of discrepancy on readers’ text processing of and memory for the sources of brief
news reports. Each story included two assertions that were attributed to different sources. We manipulated whether the second
assertion was either discrepant or consistent with the first assertion. On the basis of the discrepancy-induced source comprehension
(D-ISC) assumption, we predicted that discrepant stories would promote deeper processing and better memory for the sources
conveying the messages, as compared to consistent stories. As predicted, readers mentioned more sources in summaries of discrepant
stories, recalled more sources, made more fixations, and displayed longer gaze times in source areas when reading discrepant
than when reading consistent stories. In Experiment 2, we found enhanced memory for source–content links for discrepant stories even when intersentential connectors were absent,
and regardless of the reading goals. Discussion was focused on discrepancies as one mechanism by which readers are prompted
to encode source–content links more deeply, as a method of integrating disparate pieces of information into a coherent mental
representation of a text. 相似文献
156.
This work suggests how much human sciences can learn from a booklet almost all cultures know, The Little Prince. It stimulates to read them in their anthropological aspects of behavior facing alterity, highlighting its value without proposing it as educational element to homogenize the different. The reflection is thus focused on the demolishing and lasting effects of European culture during its colonization of American and African people. The contribution ends with Ferraioli and De Vitoria's considerations on a society based on the axiom of the free circulation of men and goods, and the legitimization of thinking given by natural law. 相似文献
157.
158.
The Flynn Effect (J. Flynn, 1987) refers to the apparent increases in intelligence quotient (IQ) observed over the past few decades. A related phenomenon is that the variance in test scores accounted for by Spearman's g (C. Spearman, 1904) varies according to IQ level. That is, g accounts for less variance in high IQ groups than in low IQ groups. Spearman termed this variant aspect of g the “law of diminishing returns.” This study extends prior research on the Flynn Effect and the law of diminishing returns by examining changes in the statistical importance of Spearman's g that may accompany secular increases in IQ. Based on the standardization data from the United States versions of the Wechsler scales (i.e., Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scales of Intelligence, D. Wechsler, 1967; Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, D. Wechsler, 1949; Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, D. Wechsler, 1955), this study indicated that, in most cases, the statistical significance of Spearman's g has indeed declined over the past several years. A. R. Jensen (1998) suggested that the components of the Flynn Effect warrant special investigation. The present study addresses this issue, in part by analyzing the Verbal and Performance subscales of the Wechsler scales. Additionally, this study further confirms the law of diminishing returns as applicable to different ages and time periods. 相似文献
159.
160.
Of 1,409 eligible children aged 6-13 years in grades 1 to 7 who were randomly selected from a national sample of Norwegian schools, 858 participated in the present study (60.9%). The sample was stratified by school centrality, region and size of grade cohort. The teachers assessed their children's academic performance, adaptive school functioning, and levels of emotional/behavioral problems using the 2001 version of the Teacher Report Form (TRF). Only one child was randomly selected from each grade cohort. Girls had significantly higher scores than boys in the Working Hard, Appropriate Behavior, Learning, and Total Adaptive Functioning domains. For girls, only the Working Hard domain was of medium effect size. While boys had significantly higher scores than girls on Attention, Thought Problems, Rule-Breaking, Aggression, Externalizing Problems and Total problems, only Attention Problems showed a medium effect size. Significant sex by age interaction effects were also found for Rule-Breaking, Externalizing, Internalizing, Anxious-Depressed and Total Problems. In all these comparisons, 10-13-year-old boys had significantly higher scores than 6-9-year olds, while girls had similar problem levels across age groups. Our mean Total Problems score (17.2) was lower than the grand mean (21.6) reported in a multi-country comparison but higher than in another Norwegian large-scale survey. Overall, our findings indicate that teachers in Scandinavia report, just as do parents, relatively low levels of emotional/behavioral problems among school-aged children. 相似文献