首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1738篇
  免费   62篇
  国内免费   1篇
  1801篇
  2023年   15篇
  2022年   15篇
  2021年   13篇
  2020年   31篇
  2019年   34篇
  2018年   45篇
  2017年   50篇
  2016年   46篇
  2015年   33篇
  2014年   37篇
  2013年   209篇
  2012年   84篇
  2011年   74篇
  2010年   44篇
  2009年   42篇
  2008年   61篇
  2007年   62篇
  2006年   43篇
  2005年   44篇
  2004年   44篇
  2003年   44篇
  2002年   53篇
  2001年   46篇
  2000年   36篇
  1999年   31篇
  1998年   26篇
  1997年   21篇
  1996年   23篇
  1995年   16篇
  1994年   21篇
  1993年   14篇
  1992年   26篇
  1991年   17篇
  1990年   14篇
  1989年   20篇
  1988年   17篇
  1987年   14篇
  1986年   14篇
  1984年   15篇
  1983年   14篇
  1981年   16篇
  1980年   18篇
  1978年   14篇
  1976年   18篇
  1975年   15篇
  1974年   15篇
  1972年   17篇
  1968年   23篇
  1967年   15篇
  1966年   20篇
排序方式: 共有1801条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
971.
Models used to analyze cross-classifications of counts from psychological experiments must represent associations between multiple discrete variables and take into account attributes of stimuli, experimental conditions, or characteristics of subjects. The models must also lend themselves to psychological interpretations about underlying structures mediating the relationship between stimuli and responses. To meet these needs, the author extends the graphical latent variable models for nominal and/or ordinal data proposed by C. J. Anderson and J. K. Vermunt (2000) to situations in which dependencies between observed variables are not fully accounted for by the latent variables. The graphical models provide a unified framework for studying multivariate associations that include log-linear models and log-multiplicative association models as special cases.  相似文献   
972.
Conditioned flavor aversion was examined in Wistar-derived albino rats that were immersed in cold water for 0, 2.5, 5, or 10 min immediately following 10-min exposure to a.1% saccharin solution and given an intraperitoneal (i.p.) injection of 0.15 M lithium chloride (LiCl) either 90, 135, 180, or 225 min later. Cold water immersion for 2.5, 5, and 10 min led to body temperature decreases of approximately 4.5, 7, and 10 degrees C, respectively. Rats whose body temperatures were not reduced (0 min immersion) showed no saccharin aversion when the LiCl was delayed 90 min. Rats whose body temperatures were reduced 4.5, 7, and 10 degrees C displayed conditioned aversions at LiCl delays up to 135, 180, and 225 min, respectively. These results were interpreted in terms of a cold-induced slowing of a biochemical clock that may uniquely govern specific timing processes involved in associative learning over long delays, such as long-trace conditioned flavor aversion, learned safety, and certain types of learning that involve an extensive time lapse (e.g., extinction of fear).  相似文献   
973.
The purpose of this study was to examine differences between children who do (CWS) and children who do not stutter (CWNS) on standardized tests of receptive/expressive language and receptive vocabulary. Subjects were 16 boys and 4 girls who stutter (mean age = 46.80 months) and 16 boys and 4 girls who do not stutter (mean age = 47.55 months). Each child was audio recorded during a loosely structured, 30-minute conversation with an adult. This conversational interaction was subsequently assessed to provide information pertinent to the child's frequency and type of speech disfluency. After completion of the adult-child conversational interaction, each child was administered and responded to standardized tests of syntactic, semantic, and phonological abilities and development. Results indicated that the difference between measures of receptive/expressive language and receptive vocabulary is significantly greater for CWS than CWNS; however, this difference between receptive/expressive language and receptive vocabulary scores was not significantly correlated with the overall stuttering frequency of CWS. Findings were taken to suggest that the semantic development of CWS may lag behind their syntactic development, a possible imbalance among components of the speech-language systems of CWS that may contribute to the difficulties they have establishing normal speech fluency. Research supported by an NIH grant (DC00523) to Vanderbilt University.  相似文献   
974.
A growing body of evidence from humans and other animals suggests the amygdala may be a critical neural substrate for emotional processing. In particular, recent studies have shown that damage to the human amygdala impairs the normal appraisal of social signals of emotion, primarily those of fear. However, effective social communication depends on both the ability to receive (emotional appraisal) and the ability to send (emotional expression) signals of emotional state. Although the role of the amygdala in the appraisal of emotion is well established, its importance for the production of emotional expressions is unknown. We report a case study of a patient with bilateral amygdaloid damage who, despite a severe deficit in interpreting facial expressions of emotion including fear, exhibits an intact ability to express this and other basic emotions. This dissociation suggests that a single neural module does not support all aspects of the social communication of emotional state.  相似文献   
975.
1980年,克丽丝·纽丝兰—沃哈德和埃里克·韦乔斯开始研究果蝇中的胚胎致死突变,这项研究后来获得了诺贝尔奖,在某种意义上,它标志着生物学一个富有魅力的理论时代的终结和缺乏想象力的事实时代的到来(这个短语出自于T.H.赫胥黎,他认为悲剧就是“富有魅力的理论被缺乏想象力的事实所扼杀”)。如果沃森和克里克的DNA双链结构模型显示了想象力(只具有不多的事实)可以超越自然,那么,纽丝兰—沃哈德和韦乔斯的饱和筛选法则表明进化能够产生如此难以想象的复杂的生物学机制,以致如果以直觉的方法来研究它们,既使不是可笑的,也是毫无用处的。…  相似文献   
976.
How effective are the Frostig training program and corrective reading instruction for improving the reading and visual-motor skills of pupils who possess deficiencies in both areas? Each of the three teachers worked each school day for sixteen weeks with the subjects of the two treatment groups plus a play placebo group. On the basis of the results of this study there is little, if any, support for the use of the Frostig program or corrective reading as treatments to improve the reading skills of children considered to possess a visual-motor problem. Due to the performances of the attention-placebo group, it is logical to hypothesize that improvements of the remedial groups are more a function of personal attention than the unique remedial powers of the two treatments. Consideration should be given to the optimal number of Frostig sessions as well as to the possibility of a ceiling effect of the Frostig program. Eight weeks of training would appear to be the maximum time before regression or improvement takes place.  相似文献   
977.
John A. Snyder     
Conclusion Wise counseling can relieve the anxiety and inner conflict of parents torn between the dread of their retarded offspring becoming a parent and the fear of an operation they believe to be illegal, immoral, and castrating.By knowing where to refer patients, clergymen can make it possible for some of these deprived members of society to find support and love within marriage without being overburdened by children they are not capable of rearing properly.  相似文献   
978.
979.
Five experiments examined effects of songs with violent lyrics on aggressive thoughts and hostile feelings. Experiments 1, 3, 4 and 5 demonstrated that college students who heard a violent song felt more hostile than those who heard a similar but nonviolent song. Experiments 2-5 demonstrated a similar increase in aggressive thoughts. These effects replicated across songs and song types (e.g., rock, humorous, nonhumorous). Experiments 3-5 also demonstrated that trait hostility was positively related to state hostility but did not moderate the song lyric effects. Discussion centers on the potential role of lyric content on aggression in short-term settings, relation to catharsis and other media violence domains, development of aggressive personality, differences between long-term and short-term effects, and possible mitigating factors.  相似文献   
980.
Emotional convergence between people over time   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
The authors propose that people in relationships become emotionally similar over time--as this similarity would help coordinate the thoughts and behaviors of the relationship partners, increase their mutual understanding, and foster their social cohesion. Using laboratory procedures to induce and assess emotional response, the authors found that dating partners (Study 1) and college roommates (Studies 2 and 3) became more similar in their emotional responses over the course of a year. Further, relationship partners with less power made more of the change necessary for convergence to occur. Consistent with the proposed benefits of emotional similarity, relationships whose partners were more emotionally similar were more cohesive and less likely to dissolve. Discussion focuses on implications of emotional convergence and on potential mechanisms.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号